Abstract
Reasoning about ecosystems includes consideration of causality over temporal and spatial distances; yet learners typically focus on immediate time frames and local contexts. Teaching students to reason beyond these boundaries has met with some success based upon tests that cue students to the types of reasoning required. Virtual worlds offer an opportunity to assess what students actually do in a simulated context. Beyond this, mobile devices make it possible to scaffold and assess learning in the real world. Situating learning outside, in the target contexts, bypasses many of the challenges of transfer. A study investigated the learning of fifth and sixth graders (n = 38) while they used a virtual world called EcoMUVE, designed to support learning of ecosystems concepts and complex causal dynamics, and mobile broadband device (MBDs) components, designed to assess and support learning and transfer in a real pond ecosystem. The experiences of two classes were contrasted as reference populations; one class participated in the MBD experience first, followed by the learning components in EcoMUVE; the other participated in EcoMUVE first, followed by the MBD components. Rich and triangulated data was collected to illuminate how students experienced and responded to the curriculum components. Both classes made learning gains in EcoMUVE. Students who completed EcoMUVE prior to their MBD experience transferred concepts to their pond explorations. Both classes made learning gains at the pond following the MBD support and revealed more expert reasoning about the importance of change over time and distant drivers in ecosystem dynamics.












Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
Accessed at http://gse.harvard.edu/uclab/.
References
Atran, S. (1995). Causal constraints on categories and categorical constraints on biological reasoning across cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 205–233). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Barab, S., & Squire, K. (2004). Design-based research: Putting a stake in the ground. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(1), 1–14.
Chi, M. T. H., & Van Lehn, K. (2012). Seeing deep structure from the interactions of surface features. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 177–188.
Danish, J. A., Peppler, K., Phelps, D., & Washington, D. (2011). Life in the hive: Supporting inquiry into complexity within the zone of proximal development. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20(5), 454–467.
Day, S. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (2012). The import of knowledge export: Connecting findings and theories of transfer of learning. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 153–176.
Dede, C. (2009). Immersive interfaces for engagement and learning. Science, 323(5910), 66–69.
Goldstone, R. L., & Sakamoto, Y. (2003). The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 414–466.
Gopnik, A., & Glymour, C. (2002). Causal maps and Bayes nets: A cognitive and computational account of theory formation. In P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 117–132). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Gopnik, A., & Schulz, L. (2007). Introduction. In A. Gopnik & L. Schulz (Eds.), Causal learning. Oxford: Oxford Press.
Grotzer, T. A., & Basca, B. B. (2003). How does grasping the underlying causal structures of ecosystems impact students’ understanding? Journal of Biological Education, 38(1), 16–29.
Grotzer, T. A., Kamarainen, A., Tutwiler, M. S., Metcalf, S., & Dede, C. (2013). Learning to reason about ecosystems dynamics over time: The challenges of an event-based causal focus. BioScience, 63(4), 288–296.
Grotzer, T. A., & Solis, S. L. (2015). Action at an attentional distance: A study of children’s reasoning about causes and effects involving spatial and attentional discontinuity. Journal for Research in Science Teaching (accepted, in revision).
Grotzer, T. A., Solis, S. L., & Honey, R. B. (2014). The power of comparison when the concept of control doesn’t apply. (Working Paper, No. 2014-2), Causal Learning in the Classroom Lab, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA: Author.
Grotzer, T. A., & Tutwiler, M. S. (2014). Simplifying causal complexity: How interactions between modes of causal induction and information availability lead to heuristic driven reasoning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8(3), 97–114.
Grotzer, T. A., Tutwiler, M. S., Dede, C. Kamarainen, A., & Metcalf, S. (2011). Helping students learn more expert framing of complex causal dynamics in ecosystems using EcoMUVE. Presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference, Orlando, FL.
Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414–434.
Harris, P. L. (2002). What do children learn from testimony? In P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 316–334). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Azevedo, R. (2006). Understanding complex systems: Some core challenges. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(1), 53–61.
Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Pfeffer, M. G. (2004). Comparing expert and novice understanding of a complex system from the perspective of structures, behaviors, and functions. Cognitive Science, 28, 127–138.
Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Marathe, S., & Liu, L. (2007). Fish swim, rocks sit, and lungs breathe: Expert-novice understanding of complex systems. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 16, 307–331.
Hogan, K., & Fisherkeller, J. (1996). Representing students’ thinking about nutrient cycling in ecosystems: Bi-dimensional coding of a complex topic. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(9), 941–970.
Jacobson, M. J. (2001). Problem-solving, cognition, and complex systems: Differences between experts and novices. Complexity, 6(3), 41–49.
Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains. In L. Hirschfield & S. Gelman (Eds.), Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234–254). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Klopfer, E., & Yoon, S. (2005). Using palm technology in participatory simulations of complex systems: A new take on ubiquitous and accessible mobile computing. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14(3), 287–295.
Metcalf, S. J., Kamarainen, A., Tutwiler, M. S., Grotzer, T. A., & Dede, C. J. (2011). Ecosystem science learning via multi-user virtual environments. International Journal of Gaming and Computer-Mediated Simulations, 3(1), 86–90.
Penner, D. (2000). Explaining systems: Investigating middle school students’ understanding of emergent phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(8), 784–806.
Perkins, D. N., Jay, E., & Tishman, S. (1993). Beyond abilities: A dispositional theory of thinking. The Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 1–21.
Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (2012). Knowledge to go: A motivational and dispositional view of transfer. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 248–258.
Raia, F. (2008). Causality in complex dynamic systems: A challenge in earth systems science education. Journal of Geoscience Education, 56(1), 81–94.
Ratterman, M. J., & Gentner, D. (1998). The effect of language on similarity: The use of relational labels improves young children’s performance in a mapping task. In K. Holyoak, D. Gentner, & B. Kokinov (Eds.), Advances in analogy research: Integration of theory and data from the cognitive, computational, and neural sciences (pp. 274–282). Sophia: New Bulgarian University.
Shepardson, D. P., Wee, B., Priddy, M., Schellenberger, L., & Harbor, J. (2007). What is a watershed? Implications of student conceptions for environmental science education and the national science education standards. Science Education, 91, 544–578.
Tal, T., Alon, N. L., & Morag, O. (2014). Exemplary practices in field trips to natural environments. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 51, 430–461.
Weathers, K., Strayer, D., & Likens, G. (2013). Fundamentals of ecosystem science. Waltham, MA: Academic Press.
Acknowledgments
We would like to express our appreciation to the teachers and students who contributed their time to this project and enabled us to collect data on their reasoning patterns. We also thank Maya Bialik, Amber K. Boyd, Maleka Donaldson Gramling, Couger Jimenez Jaramillo, Tim Johnson, Miles Malbrough, Daniel Oh, S. Lynneth Solis and M. Shane Tutwiler for their assistance in data collection, transcribing, and analysis. Thank you to Daniel Hackett, Brandon Pousley, and the Cambridge Water Department for their assistance in developing components for EcoMOBILE. This work is supported by the National Science Foundation, Grant No. DRK12 1118530 to Chris Dede and Tina Grotzer. All opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. EcoMUVE was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, Grant No. R305A080514 to Chris Dede and Tina Grotzer. All opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Institute for Education Sciences.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Grotzer, T.A., Powell, M.M., M. Derbiszewska, K. et al. Turning Transfer Inside Out: The Affordances of Virtual Worlds and Mobile Devices in Real World Contexts for Teaching About Causality Across Time and Distance in Ecosystems. Tech Know Learn 20, 43–69 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-014-9241-5
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-014-9241-5