Abstract
The present study evaluates the cognitive representation of a kicking movement performed by a human and a humanoid robot, and how they are represented in experts and novices of soccer and robotics, respectively. To learn about the expertise-dependent development of memory structures, we compared the representation structures of soccer experts and robot experts concerning a human and humanoid robot kicking movement. We found different cognitive representation structures for both expertise groups under two different motor performance conditions (human vs. humanoid robot). In general, the expertise relies on the perceptual-motor knowledge of the human motor system. Thus, the soccer experts’ cognitive representation of the humanoid robot movement is dominated by their representation of the corresponding human movement. Additionally, our results suggest that robot experts, in contrast to soccer experts, access functional features of the technical system of the humanoid robot in addition to their perceptual-motor knowledge about the human motor system. Thus, their perceptual-motor and neuro-functional machine representation are integrated into a cognitive representation of the humanoid robot movement.




Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Abeele, S., & Bock, O. (2003). Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories. Experimental Brain Research, 148(1), 128–132.
Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 1109–1116. doi:10.1038/nn.2182.
Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(8), 561–567.
Bläsing, B., Tenenbaum, G., & Schack, T. (2009). The cognitive structure of movements in classical dance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10(3), 350–360.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.
Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(22), 1905–1910. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363–406. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363.
Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2004). Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues. Psychological Research, 69(1–2), 147–156. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0165-2.
Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769–771.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grezès, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1–19.
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14(1), 103–109. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0832.
Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13(6), 522–525.
Knoblich, G., & Jordan, J. S. (2003). Action coordination in groups and individuals: Learning anticipatory control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 1006–1016. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.1006.
Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 456–465. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.456.
Lees, A., & Nolan, L. (1998). The biomechanics of soccer: A review. Journal of Sport Science, 16(3), 211–234.
Lex, H., Weigelt, M., Knoblauch, A., & Schack, T. (2012). Functional relationship between cognitive representations of movement directions and visuomotor adaptation performance. Experimental Brain Research, 223(4), 457–467.
Lex, H., Weigelt, M., Knoblauch, A., & Schack, T. (2014). The functional role of cognitive frameworks on visuomotor adaptation performance. Journal of Motor Behavior, 46(6), 389–396. doi:10.1080/00222895.2014.920290.
Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 89–115. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.000513.
Meulenbroek, R. G., Rosenbaum, D. A., Thomassen, A. J., Loukopoulos, L. D., & Vaughan, J. (1996). Adaptation of a reaching model to handwriting: How different effectors can produce the same written output, and other results. Psychological Research, 59(1), 64–74.
Pfeifer, R., & Bongard, J. (2007). How the body shapes the way we think: A new view of intelligence. Cambidge: The MIT Press.
Pfeifer, R., Lungarella, M., & Iida, F. (2007). Self-organization, embodiment, and biologically inspired robotics. Science, 318(5853), 1088–1093.
Press, C. (2011). Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and anthropomorphism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(6), 1410–1418.
Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(7), 576–582.
Rosenbaum, D. A., Carlson, R. A., & Gilmore, R. O. (2001). Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 453–470.
Schack, T. (2003). The relationship between motor representation and biomechanical parameters in complex movements: Towards an integrative perspective of movement science. European Journal of Sport Science, 3(2), 1–13. doi:10.1080/17461390300073201.
Schack, T. (2004). The cognitive architecture of complex movement. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2(4), 403–438.
Schack, T. (2011). Measuring mental representations. In G. Tenenbaum, R. C. Eklund, & A. Kamata (Eds.), Handbook of measurement in sport and exercise psychology (Vol. 1). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Schack, T., Bläsing, B., Hughes, C. M. L, Flash, T., & Schilling, M. (2014). Elements and construction of motor control. In: A. G. Papaioannou & D. Hackfort (Eds.), Routledge companion to sport and exercise psychology: Global perspectives and fundamental concepts (pp. 308–323). New York: Routledge.
Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. (2006). Representation of motor skills in human long-term memory. Neuroscience Letters, 391(3), 77–81. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.009.
Schack, T., & Ritter, H. (2009). The cognitive nature of action—Functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science, and robotics. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 231–250. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)01319-3.
Schack, T., & Ritter, H. (2013). Representation and learning in motor action—Bridges between experimental research and cognitive robotics. New Ideas in Psychology, 31(3), 258–269. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.04.003.
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2005). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70–76. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009.
Ward, P., & Williams, A. M. (2003). Perceptual and cognitive skill development in soccer: The multidimensional nature of expert performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25(1), 93–111.
Weigelt, M., Ahlmeyer, T., Lex, H., & Schack, T. (2011). The cognitive representation of a throwing technique in judo experts—Technological ways for individual skill diagnostics in high-performance sports. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12(3), 231–235.
Acknowledgments
This work gratefully acknowledges the financial support from Honda Research Institute Europe for the project: Cognitive planning and motor adaptation in manual action. We thank the reviewers for their critical comments and suggestions, which helped us to improve the manuscript substantially.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lex, H., Schütz, C., Knoblauch, A. et al. Cognitive Representation of a Complex Motor Action Executed by Different Motor Systems. Minds & Machines 25, 1–15 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-014-9351-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-014-9351-9