Abstract
Recognizing threats is a vital ability in social interactions across the animal kingdom. Yet, the role of specific perceptual elements, especially the head-body silhouette in aversive situations, remains relatively unexplored. In our study, we investigated the modulation of defensive behavior in rats facing a four-wheeled robot designed to simulate a natural predator. The robot featured an inflatable top allowing instant changes in appearance. In Experiment 1, rats encountered the head-inflatable robot (HEAD) in two sessions – a training session, where the rats were sequentially chased by the robot in both head-deflated (HEAD-Off) and head-inflated (HEAD-On) states, and a test session with a stationary HEAD-On or HEAD-Off robot 3 weeks later to assess long-term behavioral changes. The rats displayed reduced velocity and exploration in the center area during the HEAD-On phase of the training session. During the test session, the rats maintained a greater distance from the stationary HEAD-On robot than from the HEAD-Off robot, indicating sustained alertness based on the memory of the previous threat encounter. In Experiment 2, an identical procedure with the body-inflatable robot (BODY) was conducted. No significant differences emerged between BODY-On and BODY-Off conditions, except for a slight reduction in movement velocity during the BODY-On phase in the training session. Considering the substantial difference in behavioral reactions to HEAD-On versus BODY-On robots, we concluded that the emergence of head-like component in a chasing robot produced heightened vigilance and alertness. Since the two types of robots adopted a minimal design and differ only by the position of the inflatable top portion, our findings highlight the significant impact of a clearly recognizable head-like component in a threat encounter. The head-body silhouette provides a key perceptual framework for designing a social robot, with implications for both animal-robot and human–robot interactions.







Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Data availability
All codes and analysis scripts used in the current study is available online at https://github.com/knowblesse/Gudaihead. Video and processed behavior data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68(4):619–640. https://doi.org/10.1139/z90-092
Lima SL (2002) Putting predators back into behavioral predator–prey interactions. Trends Ecol Evol 17(2):70–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02393-X
Rogan MT, LeDoux JE (1996) Emotion: systems, cells, synaptic plasticity. Cell 85(4):469–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81247-7
Mobbs D et al (2009) From threat to fear: the neural organization of defensive fear systems in humans. J Neurosci 29(39):12236–12243. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-09.2009
Halloy J et al (2007) Social integration of robots into groups of cockroaches to control self-organized choices. Science 318(5853):1155–1158. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144259
Spinello C, Yang Y, Macrì S, Porfiri M (2019) Zebrafish adjust their behavior in response to an interactive robotic predator. Front Robot AI 6:38. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2019.00038
Shi Q et al (2015) Behavior modulation of rats to a robotic rat in multi-rat interaction. Bioinspir Biomim 10(5):056011. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-3190/10/5/056011
R. Du, Z. Li, K. Youcef-Toumi, and P. Valdivia y Alvarado, Eds., Robot fish: bio-inspired fishlike underwater robots. In Springer tracts in mechanical engineering. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer, 2015.
Siddall R (2023) Ethorobotic rats for rodent behavioral research: design considerations. Front Behav Neurosci 17:1281494. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1281494
Romano D, Benelli G, Stefanini C (2023) How aggressive interactions with biomimetic agents optimize reproductive performances in mass-reared males of the Mediterranean fruit fly. Biol Cybern 117(3):249–258. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-023-00965-w
Horsevad N, Kwa HL, Bouffanais R (2022) Beyond bio-inspired robotics: how multi-robot systems can support research on collective animal behavior. Front Robot AI 9:865414. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2022.865414
Robins B, Dautenhahn K, Boekhorst RT, Billard A (2005) Robotic assistants in therapy and education of children with autism: can a small humanoid robot help encourage social interaction skills? Univers Access Inf Soc 4(2):105–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10209-005-0116-3
Stolarz M, Mitrevski A, Wasil M, Plöger PG (2024) Learning-based personalisation of robot behaviour for robot-assisted therapy. Front Robot AI 11:1352152. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2024.1352152
Dubois-Sage M, Jacquet B, Jamet F, Baratgin J (2024) People with Autism spectrum disorder could interact more easily with a robot than with a human: reasons and limits. Behav Sci 14(2):131. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020131
Frohnwieser A, Murray JC, Pike TW, Wilkinson A (2016) Using robots to understand animal cognition: Robots in animal cognition. J Exp Anal Behav 105(1):14–22. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.193
Kaplan F, Oudeyer P-Y, Kubinyi E, Miklósi A (2002) Robotic clicker training. Robot Auton Syst 38(3–4):197–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0921-8890(02)00168-9
Khurshid J, Bing-Rong H. Military robots-a glimpse from today and tomorrow. In: ICARCV 2004 8th Control, Automation, Robotics and Vision Conference, 2004. 2004 Dec 6 (Vol. 1, pp. 771-777). IEEE.https://doi.org/10.1109/ICARCV.2004.1468925
Voth D (2004) A new generation of military robots. IEEE Intell Syst 19(4):2–3. https://doi.org/10.1109/MIS.2004.30
Budiharto W, Irwansyah E, Suroso JS, Gunawan AAS (2020) Design of object tracking for military robot using PID controller and computer vision. ICIC International Conference. https://doi.org/10.24507/icicel.14.03.289
Lester LS, Fanselow MS (1985) Exposure to a cat produces opioid analgesia in rats. Behav Neurosci 99(4):756–759. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.99.4.756
Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Agullana R, Weiss SM (1991) Twenty-two kHz alarm cries to presentation of a predator, by laboratory rats living in visible burrow systems. Physiol Behav 50(5):967–972. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(91)90423-L
Choi JS, Kim JJ (2010) Amygdala regulates risk of predation in rats foraging in a dynamic fear environment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U A 107(50):21773–21777. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010079108
Lee J-H, Kimm S, Han J-S, Choi J-S (2018) Chasing as a model of psychogenic stress: characterization of physiological and behavioral responses. Stress 21(4):323–332. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2018.1455090
X. Meng, S. Wang, Z. Cao, and L. Zhang, “A review of quadruped robots and environment perception,” In: 2016 35th Chinese Control Conference (CCC), Chengdu: IEEE, Jul. 2016, pp. 6350–6356. https://doi.org/10.1109/ChiCC.2016.7554355.
Y. Li, B. Li, J. Ruan, and X. Rong, “Research of mammal bionic quadruped robots: A review,” In: 2011 IEEE 5th International Conference on Robotics, Automation and Mechatronics (RAM), Qingdao, China: IEEE, Sep. 2011, pp. 166–171. https://doi.org/10.1109/RAMECH.2011.6070476.
Zambetti PR, Schuessler BP, Lecamp BE, Shin A, Kim EJ, Kim JJ (2022) Ecological analysis of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Commun Biol 5(1):830. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03802-1
Diehl MM, Bravo-Rivera C, Quirk GJ (2019) The study of active avoidance: a platform for discussion. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 107:229–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.010
Niesink RJM, Van Ree JM (1982) Short-term isolation increases social interactions of male rats: a parametric analysis. Physiol Behav 29(5):819–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(82)90331-6
Bartal IB-A, Decety J, Mason P (2011) Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science 334(6061):1427–1430. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1210789
Ben-Ami Bartal I, Rodgers DA, Bernardez Sarria MS, Decety J, Mason P (2014) Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. Elife 14(3):e01385. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01385
Panksepp J, Herman BH, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi FG (1980) Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4(4):473–487. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6
Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS (1997) Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res 88(2):261–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00088-0
Totty MS, Payne MR, Maren S (2019) Event boundaries do not cause the immediate extinction deficit after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Sci Rep 9(1):9459. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46010-4
Kosten TA, Lee HJ, Kim JJ (2006) Early life stress impairs fear conditioning in adult male and female rats. Brain Res 1087(1):142–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.009
C. Belzung, “Chapter 4.11 Measuring rodent exploratory behavior,” in Techniques in the Behavioral and Neural Sciences, vol. 13, Elsevier, 1999, pp. 738–749. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0921-0709(99)80057-1.
Fanselow MS (1980) Conditional and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. Pavlov J Biol Sci 15(4):177–182
Book DL, Freeberg TM (2015) Titmouse calling and foraging are affected by head and body orientation of cat predator models and possible experience with real cats. Anim Cogn 18(5):1155–1164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0888-7
Carter J, Lyons NJ, Cole HL, Goldsmith AR (2008) Subtle cues of predation risk: starlings respond to a predator’s direction of eye-gaze. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 275(1644):1709–1715. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0095
Burger J, Gochfeld M, Murray BG (1992) Risk discrimination of eye contact and directness of approach in black iguanas (Ctenosaura similis). J Comp Psychol 106(1):97–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.106.1.97
Whitham W, Schapiro SJ, Troscianko J, Yorzinski JL (2022) The gaze of a social monkey is perceptible to conspecifics and predators but not prey. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 289(1976):20220194. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0194
J.-H. Kim, S.-H. Choi, D. Kim, J. Kim, and M. Cho, “Animal-Robot Interaction for pet caring,” In: 2009 IEEE International Symposium on Computational Intelligence in Robotics and Automation - (CIRA), Daejeon, Korea (South): IEEE, Dec. 2009, pp. 159–164. https://doi.org/10.1109/CIRA.2009.5423214.
C. W. Park, J.-H. Seon, J.-H. Kim, and J.-H. Kim, “Pet Care Robot for Playing with Canines,” In: Robot Intelligence Technology and Applications 4, vol. 447, J.-H. Kim, F. Karray, J. Jo, P. Sincak, and H. Myung, Eds., in Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing, vol. 447. , Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2017, pp. 299–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-31293-4_24.
Koh WQ, Hoel V, Casey D, Toomey E (2023) Strategies to implement pet robots in long-term care facilities for dementia care: a modified delphi study. J Am Med Dir Assoc 24(1):90–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2022.09.010
Ihamäki P, Heljakka K (2024) Robot pets as ‘serious toys’- activating social and emotional experiences of elderly people. Inf Syst Front 26(1):25–39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10796-021-10175-z
Desimone R (1996) Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci 93(24):13494–13499. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494
Samal A, Iyengar PA (1992) Automatic recognition and analysis of human faces and facial expressions: a survey. Pattern Recognit 25(1):65–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-3203(92)90007-6
Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S (2001) Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology 56(2):153–158. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.56.2.153
Noroozi F, Corneanu CA, Kaminska D, Sapinski T, Escalera S, Anbarjafari G (2021) Survey on emotional body gesture recognition. IEEE Trans Affect Comput 12(2):505–523. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2018.2874986
Tinbergen N (1948) Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bull 1(60):6–51
Schürg-Pfeiffer E, Spreckelsen C, Ewert J-P (1993) Temporal discharge patterns of tectal and medullary neurons chronically recorded during snapping toward prey in toads Bufo bufo spinosus. J Comp Physiol A 173(3):363–376. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00212701
Desimone R, Albright T, Gross C, Bruce C (1984) Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque. J Neurosci 4(8):2051–2062. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-08-02051.1984
Kim EJ, Park M, Kong M-S, Park SG, Cho J, Kim JJ (2015) Alterations of hippocampal place cells in foraging rats facing a ‘Predatory’ Threat. Curr Biol 25(10):1362–1367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.048
T. D. Hoa and J.-J. Cabibihan, “Cute and soft: baby steps in designing robots for children with autism,” In: Proceedings of the Workshop at SIGGRAPH Asia, Singapore Singapore: ACM, Nov. 2012, pp. 77–79. https://doi.org/10.1145/2425296.2425310.
M. Paetzel, G. Perugia, and G. Castellano, “The Persistence of First Impressions: The Effect of Repeated Interactions on the Perception of a Social Robot,” in Proceedings of the 2020 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Cambridge United Kingdom: ACM, Mar. 2020, pp. 73–82. https://doi.org/10.1145/3319502.3374786.
A. K. Fiolka, M. Donnermann, and B. Lugrin, “Investigating the Mere Exposure Effect in Relation to Perceived Eeriness and Humaneness of a Social Robot,” in Companion of the 2024 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Boulder CO USA: ACM, Mar. 2024, pp. 453–457. https://doi.org/10.1145/3610978.3640665.
Acknowledgements
Part of this work utilized ChatGPT (v4) in grammar correction and refining the clarity of the writing.
Funding
This research is funded by National Research Foundation of Korea (2021M3E5D2A01023887).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed to the study’s conceptualization and writing. JJ: robot hardware/software design and building, data analysis. KJ: investigation, animal surgery and experimentation. JC: supervision, reviewing and editing, and funding acquisition. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Ethical approval
All experiment procedures involving animals were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Korea University (KUIACUC-2023–0050).
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Supplementary file1 (MP4 80977 KB)
Supplementary file2 (MP4 79157 KB)
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Jeong, J.H., Jo, K.I. & Choi, JS. A “head-like” component of a terrestrial robot promotes anxiety-like and defensive behaviors. Intel Serv Robotics 17, 1019–1030 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11370-024-00552-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11370-024-00552-3