Abstract
Understanding people’s perceptions and inferences about social robots and, thus, their responses toward them, constitutes one of the most pervasive research themes in the field of Human–Robot interaction today. We herein augment and extend this line of work by investigating, for the first time, the novel proposition that one’s implicit self-theory orientation (underlying beliefs about the malleability of self-attributes, such as one’s intelligence), can influence one’s perceptions of emerging social robots developed for everyday use. We show that those who view self-attributes as fixed (entity theorists) express greater robot anxiety than those who view self-attributes as malleable (incremental theorists). This result holds even when controlling for well-known covariate influences, like prior robot experience, media exposure to science fiction, technology commitment, and certain demographic factors. However, only marginal effects were obtained for both attitudinal and intentional robot acceptance, respectively. In addition, we show that incremental theorists respond more favorably to social robots, compared to entity theorists. Furthermore, we find evidence indicating that entity theorists exhibit more favorable responses to a social robot positioned as a servant. We conclude with a discussion about our findings.


Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
The term “implicit self-theories” is used herein, rather than “mindset,” as the latter is an ambiguous term with numerous conceptualisations and diverse meanings (see, for example, [51, 54, 103, 131]). Accordingly, the terms “incremental theories” and “entity theories,” are used here, instead of “fixed” and “growth” mindsets (see also [147]).
Following Bernotat and Eyssel [11], attitudinal and intentional robot acceptance were assessed as different dimensions of robot acceptance. According to Ezer [46], attitudinal robot acceptance refers to one’s positive beliefs regarding a robot more generally, whereas intentional acceptance refers to an individual’s intention to purchase or use a robot.
For the purposes of this study, the role of “assistant” was used rather than “partner.” The rationale for this is the empirical evidence on robot role, which has repeatedly shown that people distinguish between the two roles (servant vs. assistant). In contrast, there appears to be much less evidence for partner role type perceptions in the HRI literature (e.g., [27]).
As described in Sect. 2.3.1, higher (vs. lower) scores on the implicit self-theories measure indicate more (vs. less) of an incremental (vs. entity) theory. Therefore, positive (vs. negative) statistically significant effects indicate an association between incremental (vs. entity) theory and the dependant variable of interest.
References
(2020) Conference Proceedings. Proceedings of the 2020 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human–Robot Interaction, ISBN 9781450367462, Association for Computing Machinery, Cambridge, UK
Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR (1991) Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Sage, Thousand Oaks
Aldahdouh TZ, Nokelainen P, Korhonen V (2018) Innovativeness of staff in higher education: Do implicit theories and goal orientations matter? Int J Higher Edu 7(2):43–57
Andrist S, Mutlu B, Tapus A (2015) Look like me: matching robot personality via gaze to increase motivation. In: Proceedings of the 33rd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems, ACM, New York, NY, USA, CHI ’15, pp 3603–3612, https://doi.org/10.1145/2702123.2702592,
Appel M, Izydorczyk D, Weber S, Mara M, Lischetzke T (2020) The uncanny of mind in a machine: Humanoid robots as tools, agents, and experiencers. Comput Hum Behav 102:274–286
Aruguete MS, Huynh H, Browne BL, Jurs B, Flint E, McCutcheon LE (2019) How serious is the carelessness problem on mechanical turk? Int J Soc Res Methodol 22(5):441–449. https://doi.org/10.1080/13645579.2018.1563966
Bartneck C, Suzuki T, Kanda T, Nomura T (2007) The influence of peoples culture and prior experiences with aibo on their attitude towards robots. Ai Soc 21(1–2):217–230
Bartneck C, Duenser A, Moltchanova E, Zawieska K (2015) Comparing the similarity of responses received from experiments conducted in amazon’s mechanical turk to experiments conducted with traditional methods. PLoS ONE 10(4):e0121595. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121595
Bartneck C, Belpaeme T, Eyssel F, Kanda T, Keijsers M, Abanovi S (2020) Human-Robot interaction: an introduction. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Beraldo G, Di Battista S, Badaloni S, Menegatti E, Pivetti M (2019) Sex differences in expectations and perception of a social robot. In: 2018 IEEE workshop on advanced robotics and its social impacts (ARSO), IEEE, pp 38–43
Bernotat J, Eyssel F (2018) Can (t) wait to have a robot at home?-japanese and german users’ attitudes toward service robots in smart homes. In: 2018 27th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN), IEEE, pp 15–22
Blackwell LS, Trzesniewski KH, Dweck CS (2007) Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Dev 78(1):246–263
Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling S (2011) Amazon’s mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspect Psychol Sci 6(1):3–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/14805-009
Burnette JL, O’Boyle EH, VanEpps EM, Pollack JM, Finkel EJ (2013) Mind-sets matter: A meta-analytic review of implicit theories and self-regulation. Psychol Bull 139(3):655
Cagiltay B, Ho HR, Michaelis JE, Mutlu B (2020) Investigating family perceptions and design preferences for an in-home robot. In: Proceedings of the interaction design and children conference, pp 229–242
Chandler J, Paolacci G, Mueller P (2013) Risks and rewards of crowdsourcing marketplaces. Springer, Berlin, pp 377–392
Chiu Cy, Dweck CS, Tong JY, Fu JH (1997a) Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. J Personal Soc Psychol 73(5):923
Chiu Cy, Hong Yy, Dweck CS (1997b) Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. J Personal Soc Psychol 73(1):19
Chmielewski M, Kucker SC (2020) An mturk crisis? shifts in data quality and the impact on study results. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 11(4):464–473
Clements WA (2000) From an implicit to an explicit theory of mind. Beyond Dissociation Int Dissociated Implicit Explicit Process 22:273
Clements WA, Perner J (1994) Implicit understanding of belief. Cognit Dev 9(4):377–395
Collins EC (2019) Drawing parallels in humanother interactions: a trans-disciplinary approach to developing humanrobot interaction methodologies. Philos Trans R Soc B 374(1771):20180433
Copleston S, Bugmann G (2008) Personal robot user expectations. Adv Commun Comput Netw Secur 5:230
Damholdt MF, Vestergaard C, Nrskov M, Hakli R, Larsen S, Seibt J (2020) Towards a new scale for assessing attitudes towards social robots: The attitudes towards social robots scale (asor). Interact Stud 21(1):24–56
Damiano L, Dumouchel P (2018) Anthropomorphism in humanrobot co-evolution. Frontiers Psychol 9(468), https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00468,
Dautenhahn K (2007) Methodology & themes of Human-Robot interaction: A growing research field. Int J Adv Robot Syst 4(1):15
Dautenhahn K, Woods S, Kaouri C, Walters ML, Koay KL, Werry I (2005) What is a robot companion-friend, assistant or butler? In: 2005 IEEE/RSJ international conference on intelligent robots and systems, IEEE, pp 1192–1197
De Castella K, Goldin P, Jazaieri H, Ziv M, Dweck CS, Gross JJ (2013) Beliefs about emotion: links to emotion regulation, well-being, and psychological distress. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 35(6):497–505
De Graaf MM, Allouch SB, Klamer T (2015) Sharing a life with harvey: exploring the acceptance of and relationship-building with a social robot. Comput Hum Behav 43:1–14
Doron J, Stephan Y, Boich J, Scanff CL (2009) Coping with examinations: exploring relationships between students’ coping strategies, implicit theories of ability, and perceived control. Br J Edu Psychol 79(3):515–528
Duffy BR (2003) Anthropomorphism and the social robot. Robot Autonom Syst 42(3–4):177–190
Dupeyrat C, Marin C (2005) Implicit theories of intelligence, goal orientation, cognitive engagement, and achievement: A test of dwecks model with returning to school adults. Contemp Edu Psychol 30(1):43–59
Dweck CS (2008) Mindset: the new psychology of success. Random House Digital, Inc
Dweck CS (2013) Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Psychology Press, Hove
Dweck CS, Leggett EL (1988) A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol Rev 95(2):256
Dweck CS, Sorich L (1999) Mastery-oriented thinking. Coping 11:232–251
Dweck CS, Yeager DS (2019) Mindsets: a view from two eras. Perspect Psychol Sci 14(3):481–496. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618804166
Dweck CS, Chiu Cy, Hong Yy (1995) Implicit theories: elaboration and extension of the model. Psychol Inq 6(4):322–333
Elliott ES, Dweck CS (1988) Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. J Pers Soc Psychol 54(1):5
Enz S, Diruf M, Spielhagen C, Zoll C, Vargas PA (2011) The social role of robots in the futureexplorative measurement of hopes and fears. Int J Social Robot 3(3):263
Erdley CA, Loomis CC, Cain KM, Dumas-Hines F (1997) Relations among children’s social goals, implicit personality theories, and responses to social failure. Dev Psychol 33(2):263
Eurobarometer S (2012) Public attitudes towards robots. Special Eurobarometer 382 European Commission
Eyssel F (2017) An experimental psychological perspective on social robotics. Robot Autonom Syst 87:363–371
Eyssel F, Hegel F (2012) (s) he’s got the look: gender stereotyping of robots 1. J Appl Soc Psychol 42(9):2213–2230
Eyssel F, De Ruiter L, Kuchenbrandt D, Bobinger S, Hegel F (2012) if you sound like me, you must be more human: on the interplay of robot and user features on Human–Robot acceptance and anthropomorphism. In: 2012 7th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), IEEE, pp 125–126
Ezer N (2008) Is a robot an appliance, teammate, or friend? age-related differences in expectations of and attitudes toward personal home-based robots. Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology
Ferrari F, Paladino MP, Jetten J (2016) Blurring humanmachine distinctions: anthropomorphic appearance in social robots as a threat to human distinctiveness. Int J Social Robot 8(2):287–302
Floridi L (2017) Robots, jobs, taxes, and responsibilities. Philos Technol 30(1):1–4
Fong LHN, Chan ICC, Law R, Ly TP (2018) The Mechanism that Links the Implicit Theories of Intelligence and Continuance of Information Technology: Evidence from the Use of Mobile Apps to Make Hotel Reservations, Springer, pp 323–335
Ford JB (2017) Amazon’s mechanical turk: a comment. J Advert 46(1):156–158
Freitas AL, Gollwitzer P, Trope Y (2004) The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. J Exp Soc Psychol 40(6):739–752
Gnambs T, Appel M (2019) Are robots becoming unpopular? changes in attitudes towards autonomous robotic systems in europe. Comput Hum Behav 93:53–61
Goetz J, Kiesler S, Powers A (2003) Matching robot appearance and behavior to tasks to improve Human–Robot cooperation. In: The 12th IEEE international workshop on robot and human interactive communication, 2003. Proceedings. ROMAN 2003., IEEE, pp 55–60
Gollwitzer PM (2012) Mindset theory of action phases. Sage Publications Ltd, Thousand Oaks, pp 526–545. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n26
Gouaillier D, Hugel V, Blazevic P, Kilner C, Monceaux J, Lafourcade P, Marnier B, Serre J, Maisonnier B (2009) Mechatronic design of nao humanoid. In: 2009 IEEE international conference on robotics and automation, IEEE, pp 769–774
de Graaf MM, Allouch SB, van Dijk JA (2016) Long-term evaluation of a social robot in real homes. Interact stud 17(3):462–491
de Graaf MMA, Ben Allouch S, van Dijk JAGM (2019) Why would i use this in my home? A model of domestic social robot acceptance. Hum Comput Interact 34(2):115–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370024.2017.1312406
Gray K, Wegner DM (2012) Feeling robots and human zombies: mind perception and the uncanny valley. Cognition 125(1):125–130
Greenwald AG, Banaji MR, Rudman LA, Farnham SD, Nosek BA, Mellott DS (2002) A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychol Rev 109(1):3
Haegele M (2016) World Robotics Service Robots, 2016. IFR Statistical Department, VDMA
Hafeez A (2019) Promoting upskilling: How a situational growth mindset increases consumers adoption of really new products. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South-Eastern Norway
Haimovitz K, Dweck CS (2017) The origins of children’s growth and fixed mindsets: new research and a new proposal. Child Dev 88(6):1849–1859
Halperin E, Russell AG, Trzesniewski KH, Gross JJ, Dweck CS (2011) Promoting the middle east peace process by changing beliefs about group malleability. Science 333(6050):1767–1769
Han B, Wang L, Li X (2019) To collaborate or serve? effects of anthropomorphized brand roles and implicit theories on consumer responses. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly p 1938965519874879
Haring KS, Silvera-Tawil D, Matsumoto Y, Velonaki M, Watanabe K (2014) Perception of an android robot in japan and australia: A cross-cultural comparison. In: International conference on social robotics, Springer, pp 166–175
Haring KS, Watanabe K, Velonaki M, Tossell CC, Finomore V (2018) Ffabthe form function attribution bias in humanrobot interaction. IEEE Trans Cognit Dev Syst 10(4):843–851
Haselhuhn MP, Schweitzer ME, Wood AM (2010) How implicit beliefs influence trust recovery. Psychol Sci 21(5):645–648
Haselhuhn MP, Schweitzer ME, Kray LJ, Kennedy JA (2017) Perceptions of high integrity can persist after deception: How implicit beliefs moderate trust erosion. J Bus Ethics 145(1):215–225
Hauser D, Paolacci G, Chandler JJ (2018) Common concerns with mturk as a participant pool: Evidence and solutions
Heslin PA, Latham GP, VandeWalle D (2005) The effect of implicit person theory on performance appraisals. J Appl Psychol 90(5):842
Hinks T (2020) Fear of robots and life satisfaction. Int J Soc Robot. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-020-00640-1
Hong Yy, Chiu Cy, Dweck CS, Sacks R (1997) Implicit theories and evaluative processes in person cognition. J Exp Soc Psychol 33(3):296–323
Hong Yy, Chiu Cy, Dweck CS, Lin DMS, Wan W (1999) Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: a meaning system approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 77(3):588
Horowitz MC (2016) Public opinion and the politics of the killer robots debate. Res Polit 3(1):2053168015627183
Horstmann AC, Bock N, Linhuber E, Szczuka JM, Stramann C, Krmer NC (2018) Do a robots social skills and its objection discourage interactants from switching the robot off? PLoS ONE 13(7):e0201581
Huang N, Zuo S, Wang F, Cai P, Wang F (2017) The dark side of malleability: incremental theory promotes immoral behaviors. Frontiers Psychol 8:1341
Huff C, Tingley D (2015) who are these people? Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of mturk survey respondents. Res Polit 2(3):2053168015604648
Jain SP, Weiten TJ (2020) Consumer psychology of implicit theories: a review and agenda. Consumer Psychol Rev 3(1):60–75
Jin CH (2013) The effects of individual innovativeness on users adoption of internet content filtering software and attitudes toward childrens internet use. Comput Hum Behav 29(5):1904–1916
Kahn PH, Reichert AL, Gary HE, Kanda T, Ishiguro H, Shen S, Ruckert JH, Gill B (2011) The new ontological category hypothesis in Human–Robot interaction. In: 2011 6th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), IEEE, pp 159–160
Kam TK (2011) Implicit theories and the trust repair process. In: 22nd annual IACM conference paper
Kees J, Berry C, Burton S, Sheehan K (2017) An analysis of data quality: professional panels, student subject pools, and amazon’s mechanical turk. J Advert 46(1):141–155
Kim HC, Kramer T (2015) Do materialists prefer the brand-as-servant? The interactive effect of anthropomorphized brand roles and materialism on consumer responses. J Consumer Res 42(2):284–299
King RB, dela Rosa ED (2019) Are your emotions under your control or not? Implicit theories of emotion predict well-being via cognitive reappraisal. Personal Individ Differ 138:177–182
Knee CR (1998) Implicit theories of relationships: assessment and prediction of romantic relationship initiation, coping, and longevity. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(2):360
Knee CR, Patrick H, Lonsbary C (2003) Implicit theories of relationships: orientations toward evaluation and cultivation. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 7(1):41–55
Kok BC, Soh H (2020) Trust in robots: challenges and opportunities. Current Robot Rep 1(4):297–309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s43154-020-00029-y
Kuo IH, Rabindran JM, Broadbent E, Lee YI, Kerse N, Stafford R, MacDonald BA (2009) Age and gender factors in user acceptance of healthcare robots. In: RO-MAN 2009-The 18th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication, IEEE, pp 214–219
Levy SR, Dweck CS (1998) Trait-versus process-focused social judgment. Soc Cognit 16(1):151–172
Levy SR, Stroessner SJ, Dweck CS (1998) Stereotype formation and endorsement: the role of implicit theories. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(6):1421
Lewis M, Sycara K, Walker P (2018) The role of trust in Human-Robot interaction. Springer, Cham, pp 135–159
Li F, Harmer P, Duncan TE, Duncan SC, Acock A, Boles S (1998) Approaches to testing interaction effects using structural equation modeling methodology. Multivar Behav Res 33(1):1–39
Liang Y, Lee SA (2017) Fear of autonomous robots and artificial intelligence: evidence from national representative data with probability sampling. Int J Social Robot 9(3):379–384
Mangels JA, Butterfield B, Lamb J, Good C, Dweck CS (2006) Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social cognitive neuroscience model. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 1(2):75–86
Mathur P, Jain SP, Hsieh MH, Lindsey CD, Maheswaran D (2013) The influence of implicit theories and message frame on the persuasiveness of disease prevention and detection advocacies. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 122(2):141–151
Mathur P, Block L, YucelAybat O (2014) The effects of goal progress cues: an implicit theory perspective. J Consumer Psychol 24(4):484–496
Mathur P, Chun HH, Maheswaran D (2016) Consumer mindsets and self-enhancement: signaling versus learning. J Consumer Psychol 26(1):142–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.06.007
Matthews G, Hancock PA, Lin J, Panganiban AR, Reinerman-Jones LE, Szalma JL, Wohleber RW (2020) Evolution and revolution: Personality research for the coming world of robots, artificial intelligence, and autonomous systems. Personality and Individual Differences p 109969
Molden DC, Dweck CS (2006) Finding “meaning” in psychology: a lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. Am Psychol 61(3):192
Montford WJ, Leary RB, Nagel DM (2019) The impact of implicit self-theories and loss salience on financial risk. J Bus Res 99:1–11
Morsunbul U (2019) Human-Robot interaction: How do personality traits affect attitudes towards robot? J Human Sci 16(2):499–504
Munger K, Luca M, Nagler J, Tucker J (2018) Everyone on mechanical turk is above a threshold of digital literacy: Sampling strategies for studying digital media effects. Report, Working Paper. https://csdp.princeton.edu/sites/csdp/files/media/munger
Murphy MC, Dweck CS (2016a) Mindsets and consumer psychology: a response. J Consumer Psychol 26(1):165–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.06.006
Murphy MC, Dweck CS (2016b) Mindsets shape consumer behavior. J Consumer Psychol 26(1):127–136
Mutlu B, Osman S, Forlizzi J, Hodgins J, Kiesler S (2006) Task structure and user attributes as elements of Human–Robot interaction design. In: ROMAN 2006-The 15th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication, IEEE, pp 74–79
Neyer FJ, Felber J, Gebhardt C (2012) Entwicklung und validierung einer kurzskala zur erfassung von technikbereitschaft. Diagnostica
Ng AS, Tong EM (2013) The relation between implicit theories of personality and forgiveness. Personal Relation 20(3):479–494
Ninomiya T, Fujita A, Suzuki D, Umemuro H (2015) Development of the multi-dimensional robot attitude scale: Constructs of peoples attitudes towards domestic robots. In: International conference on social robotics, Springer, pp 482–491
Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2004) Psychology in Human–Robot communication: An attempt through investigation of negative attitudes and anxiety toward robots. In: RO-MAN 2004. 13th IEEE International workshop on robot and human interactive communication (IEEE Catalog No. 04TH8759), IEEE, pp 35–40
Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2009) Age differences and images of robots: social survey in japan. Interact Stud 10(3):374–391
Park JK, John DR (2010) Got to get you into my life: Do brand personalities rub off on consumers? J Consumer Res 37(4):655–669
Park JK, John DR (2012) Capitalizing on brand personalities in advertising: The influence of implicit self-theories on ad appeal effectiveness. J Consumer Psychol 22(3):424–432
Park JK, John DR (2018) Developing brand relationships after a brand transgression: the role of implicit theories of relationships. J Assoc Consumer Res 3(2):175–187
Plaks JE, Stecher K (2007) Unexpected improvement, decline, and stasis: a prediction confidence perspective on achievement success and failure. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(4):667
Plaks JE, Stroessner SJ, Dweck CS, Sherman JW (2001) Person theories and attention allocation: preferences for stereotypic versus counterstereotypic information. J Pers Soc Psychol 80(6):876
Price LL, Coulter RA, Strizhakova Y, Schultz AE (2017) The fresh start mindset: transforming consumers lives. J Consumer Res 45(1):21–48
Priester JR, Petty RE (2016) A research dialogue on mindsets. J Consumer Psychol 26(1):125–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.06.016
Quintanilla VD (2011) Judicial mindsets: the social psychology of implicit theories and the law. Neb L Rev 90:611
Rai D, Lin CWW (2019) The influence of implicit self-theories on consumer financial decision making. J Bus Res 95:316–325
Ray C, Mondada F, Siegwart R (2008) What do people expect from robots? In: 2008 IEEE/RSJ international conference on intelligent robots and systems, IEEE, pp 3816–3821
Reich-Stiebert N, Eyssel F (2015) Learning with educational companion robots? toward attitudes on education robots, predictors of attitudes, and application potentials for education robots. Int J Social Robot 7(5):875–888. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-015-0308-9
Reich-Stiebert N, Eyssel F, Hohnemann C (2019) Involve the user! changing attitudes toward robots by user participation in a robot prototyping process. Comput Hum Behav 91:290–296
Robb DA, Ahmad MI, Tiseo C, Aracri S, McConnell AC, Page V, Dondrup C, Chiyah Garcia FJ, Nguyen HN, Pairet E (2020) Robots in the danger zone: exploring public perception through engagement. In: Proceedings of the 2020 ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction, pp 93–102
Robert L, Alahmad R, Esterwood C, Kim S, You S, Zhang Q (2020) A review of personality in humanrobot interactions. Available at SSRN 3528496
Robinette P, Howard AM, Wagner AR (2017) Effect of robot performance on humanrobot trust in time-critical situations. IEEE Trans Hum Mach Syst 47(4):425–436. https://doi.org/10.1109/THMS.2017.2648849
Robins RW, Pals JL (2002) Implicit self-theories in the academic domain: implications for goal orientation, attributions, affect, and self-esteem change. Self Identity 1(4):313–336
Robinson J, Rosenzweig C, Moss AJ, Litman L (2019) Tapped out or barely tapped? recommendations for how to harness the vast and largely unused potential of the mechanical turk participant pool. PLoS ONE 14(12)
Rossi A, Dautenhahn K, Koay KL, Saunders J (2017) Investigating human perceptions of trust in robots for safe hri in home environments. In: Proceedings of the companion of the 2017 ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction, pp 375–376
Rossi A, Dautenhahn K, Koay KL, Walters ML (2018) The impact of peoples personal dispositions and personalities on their trust of robots in an emergency scenario. Paladyn J Behav Robot 9(1):137–154
Rossi A, Dautenhahn K, Koay KL, Walters ML, Holthaus P (2020) Evaluating peoples perceptions of trust in a robot in a repeated interactions study. In: International conference on social robotics, Springer, pp 453–465
Rucker DD, Galinsky AD (2016) Growing beyond growth: Why multiple mindsets matter for consumer behavior. J Consum Psychol 26(1):161–164
Rueben M, Nikolaidis S, de Graaf M, Phillips E, Robert L, Sirkin D, Kwon M, Thellman S (2020) Half day workshop on mental models of robots. In: Companion of the 2020 ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction, pp 658–659
Sandoval EB, Mubin O, Obaid M (2014) Human robot interaction and fiction: a contradiction. In: International conference on social robotics, Springer, pp 54–63
Santamaria T, Nathan-Roberts D (2017) Personality measurement and design in Human-Robot interaction: a systematic and critical review. In: Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting, SAGE Publications Sage CA, Los Angeles, CA 61:853–857
Schaefer KE (2016) Measuring trust in human robot interactions: development of the trust perception scale-HRI. Springer, pp 191–218
Schermerhorn P, Scheutz M, Crowell CR (2008) Robot social presence and gender: Do females view robots differently than males? In: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on Human robot interaction, ACM, pp 263–270
Severson RL, Carlson SM (2010) Behaving as or behaving as if? Childrens conceptions of personified robots and the emergence of a new ontological category. Neural Netw 23(8–9):1099–1103
Sevincer AT, Kluge L, Oettingen G (2014) Implicit theories and motivational focus: desired future versus present reality. Motiv Emot 38(1):36–46
Sharifi SS, Palmeira M (2017) Customers’ reactions to technological products: The impact of implicit theories of intelligence. Comput Hum Behav 77:309–316
Solberg E, Traavik LE, Wong SI (2020) Digital mindsets: recognizing and leveraging individual beliefs for digital transformation. California Management Review p 0008125620931839
Song YA, Lee SY, Kim Y (2019) Does mindset matter for using social networking sites?: understanding motivations for and uses of instagram with growth versus fixed mindset. International Journal of Advertising pp 1–19
Strait M, Urry HL, Muentener P (2019) Children’s responding to humanlike agents reflects an uncanny valley. In: 2019 14th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), IEEE, pp 506–515
Strait MK, Aguillon C, Contreras V, Garcia N (2017) The public’s perception of humanlike robots: Online social commentary reflects an appearance-based uncanny valley, a general fear of a technology takeover and the unabashed sexualization of female-gendered robots. In: 2017 26th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN), IEEE, pp 1418–1423
Takayama L, Ju W, Nass C (2008) Beyond dirty, dangerous and dull: what everyday people think robots should do. In: 2008 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), IEEE, pp 25–32
Tamir M, John OP, Srivastava S, Gross JJ (2007) Implicit theories of emotion: Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. J Pers Soc Psychol 92(4):731
Walters ML, Koay KL, Syrdal DS, Dautenhahn K, Te Boekhorst R (2009) Preferences and perceptions of robot appearance and embodiment in Human–Robot interaction trials. Procs of New Frontiers in Human–Robot Interaction
Wheeler SC, Omair A (2016) Potential growth areas for implicit theories research. J Consumer Psychol 26(1):137–141
Wullenkord R, Eyssel F (2020) The influence of robot number on robot group perceptiona call for action. ACM THRI 9(4):1–14
Xia Y, LeTendre G (2020) Robots for future classrooms: A cross-cultural validation study of negative attitudes toward robots scale in the us context. Int J Soc Robot pp 1–12
Xu K (2019) First encounter with robot alpha: How individual differences interact with vocal and kinetic cues in users social responses. New Media Society p 1461444819851479
Yogeeswaran K, Zotowski J, Livingstone M, Bartneck C, Sumioka H, Ishiguro H (2016) The interactive effects of robot anthropomorphism and robot ability on perceived threat and support for robotics research. J Hum Robot Interact 5(2):29–47
Yorkston EA, Nunes JC, Matta S (2010) The malleable brand: the role of implicit theories in evaluating brand extensions. J Market 74(1):80–93
Zotowski J, Yogeeswaran K, Bartneck C (2017) Can we control it? autonomous robots threaten human identity, uniqueness, safety, and resources. Int J Hum Comput Stud 100:48–54
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the University of Canterbury, New Zealand.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Allan, D.D., Vonasch, A.J. & Bartneck, C. The Doors of Social Robot Perception: The Influence of Implicit Self-theories. Int J of Soc Robotics 14, 127–140 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-021-00767-9
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-021-00767-9