Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of worked examples on mental model progression in a computer-based simulation learning environment

  • Published:
Journal of Computing in Higher Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a diagnostic problem solving operation of a computer-simulated chemical plant, chemical engineering students were randomly assigned to two groups: one studying product-oriented worked examples, the other practicing conventional problem solving. Effects of these instructional strategies on the progression of learners’ mental models were examined by comparing representations of their mental models with those of experts at three segments of the instruction. Progression of mental models for the worked example group was significantly greater than those using the problem-solving strategy. However, this progression did not manifest itself in learners’ troubleshooting performance measured by number of correct diagnosis and first time correct diagnosis. The implications of these results for designing instruction tailored to learners’ domain knowledge are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The simulation software for this research, PC-Distiller 1.0, was modeled after DISTILLER-I, a water-alcohol distillation simulation developed by De Croock and Betlem (1999) for use on Macintosh computers with permission from University of Twente, The Netherlands. The original software was redesigned by The Learning Systems Institute at Florida State University in the US to add new features and to adapt it for use on Windows operating systems.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. W. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973a). The mind’s eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 215–281). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973b). Perceptions in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 347–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darabi, A., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2007). Acquisition of troubleshooting skills in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(4), 1809–1819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobet, F. (2005). Chunking models of expertise: Implications for education. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Ionas, I. G. (2008). Designing effective supports for causal reasoning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 56, 287–308. doi:10.1007/s11423-006-9021-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 579–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & The PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 3–44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1994). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: Instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seel, N. M. (2001). Epistemology, situated cognition, and mental models: ‘Like a bridge over troubled water’. Instructional Science, 29, 403–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seel, N. M., Darabi, A. A., & Nelson, D. W. (2006). A dynamic mental model approach to examine schema development in performing a complex troubleshooting task: Retention of mental models. Technology, Instruction, Cognition, and Learning, 4(4), 273–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E. (1989). Toward assessment of cognitive and conative structures in learning. Educational Researcher, 18(9), 8–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition & Instruction, 2(1), 59–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32(1–2), 83–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1997). Training complex cognitive skills: A four-component instructional design model. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Clark, R. E., & de Croock, M. B. M. (2002). Blueprints to complex learning: The 4C/ID model. Educational Technology Research and Development, 50(2), 39–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a learner’s mind: Instructional design for complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aubteen Darabi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Darabi, A., Nelson, D.W., Meeker, R. et al. Effect of worked examples on mental model progression in a computer-based simulation learning environment. J Comput High Educ 22, 135–147 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12528-010-9033-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12528-010-9033-4

Keywords

Navigation