Abstract
In expressing emotional states, we generate a synaesthesic experience in our interlocutor when we transmit information about our feelings by the simultaneous use of several sensorial channels. These are referred to as verbal (the semantic content of the message) and non-verbal (gesture, gaze, tonal expression) modalities. From an engineering point of view the transmission of the same information by more than one sensorial channel should be considered as redundant. Is this true or does each channel transmit a specific piece of information? How much emotional information is transmitted by each channel and which plays a major role? As an attempt to address these questions, here we present a comparative analysis of the subjective perception of emotional states by the visual and auditory channels considered either singularly or in combination, always in the non-verbal modality. The results reveal that the audio and visual components of emotional messages convey much the same amount of information either separately or in combination, hence suggesting that each channel performs a robust encoding of the emotional features. Redundancy probably facilitates the recovery of emotional information in case one of the channels is impaired. This conclusion is challenged by language cultural specificity, since when tested on a foreign language the addressers rely more on visual information.




Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
In the following it is assumed that “A gesture is a movement of the body, or any part of it, that is considered as expressive of thought or feeling” (Oxford English Dictionary).
The results discussed do not consider all the range of emotional labels. Sarcasm/Irony is discarded because small children were involved in the experiments. Children were less skilled in the use of language lexicon and in perceiving subtle changes of prosodic features that characterize this emotion and therefore they may not have been able to identify it.
Because of the difficulty to retrieve video-clips where the semantic meaning of the sentences expressed by the protagonists was not clearly expressing “disgust” at a moderate intensity level, also this emotion was discarded. The video clips retrieved were expressing too obvious expressions of disgust and therefore did not allow to verify the perceptual ability of the subjects to catch this emotional feeling.
Considered reactions to goal-relevant changes in the environment and therefore associated to basic survival problems.
References
Banse R, Scherer K. Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70(3):614–36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614.
Burns KL, Beier EG. Significance of vocal and visual channels in the decoding of emotional meaning. J Commun. 1973;23:118–30. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1973.tb00936.x.
Caccioppo JT, Bush LK, Tassinary LG. Microexpressive facial actions as a functions of affective stimuli: replication and extension. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1992;18:515–26. doi:10.1177/0146167292185001.
Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Larsen JT, Poehlmann KM, Ito TA. The psychophysiology of emotion. In: Lewis JM, Haviland-Jones M, editors. Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. p. 173–91.
Ekman P. The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In: Wagner H, Manstead A, editors. Handbook of social psychophysiology. Chichester: Wiley; 1989. p. 143–64.
Ekman P. Facial expression of emotion: new findings, new questions. Psychol Sci. 1992;3:34–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00253.x.
Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot. 1992;6:169–200. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
Ekman P, Friesen WV, Hager JC. The facial action coding system. 2nd ed. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson; 2002. eBook.
Esposito A. The amount of information on emotional states conveyed by the verbal and nonverbal channels: some perceptual data. In: Stilianou Y, et al., editors. Progress in nonlinear speech processing. Lecture notes in computer science, vol. 4392. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2007. p. 245–64.
Graham J, Ricci-Bitti PE, Argyle M. A cross-cultural study of the communication of emotion by facial and gestural cues. J Hum Mov Stud. 1975;1:68–77.
Izard CE. Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion–cognition relations. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:561–5. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.561.
Izard CE. Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:288–99. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.288.
Izard CE, Ackerman BP. Motivational, organizational, and regulatory functions of discrete emotions. In: Lewis JM, Haviland-Jones M, editors. Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. p. 253–64.
Massaro DW. Perceiving talking faces. The MIT Press; 1998.
Oatley K, Jenkins JM. Understanding emotions. 2nd ed. Oxford, England: Blackwell; 2000.
Plutchik R. Emotion and their vicissitudes: emotions and psychopathology. In: Lewis JM, Haviland-Jones M, editors. Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. p. 53–66.
Scherer K. Vocal communication of emotion: a review of research paradigms. Speech Commun. 2003;40:227–56. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00084-5.
Scherer KR, Banse R, Wallbott HG, Goldbeck T. Vocal cues in emotion encoding and decoding. Motiv Emot. 1991;15:123–48. doi:10.1007/BF00995674.
Scherer KR, Banse R, Wallbott HG. Emotion inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2001;32:76–92. doi:10.1177/0022022101032001009.
Schwartz GE, Fair PL, Salt P, Mandel MR, Klerman GL. Facial muscle patterning to affective imagery in depressed and non-depressed subjects. Science. 1976;192:489–91. doi:10.1126/science.1257786.
Schwartz GE, Ahern GL, Brown S. Lateralized facial muscle response to positive and negative emotional stimuli. Psychophysiology. 1979;16:561–71. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01521.x.
Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosom Med. 1981;43:343–64.
Acknowledgments
The paper has been partially supported by COST Action 2102: “Cross Modal Analysis of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication” (www.cost.esf.org/domains_actions/ict/Actions/Verbal_and_Non-verbal_Communication) and by Regione Campania, L.R. N.5 del 28.03.2002, Project ID N. BRC1293, Feb. 2006. Deep gratitude goes to Professor Roberto Ligrone for his useful comments and suggestions. Tina Marcella Nappi is also acknowledged for her editorial help.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Esposito, A. The Perceptual and Cognitive Role of Visual and Auditory Channels in Conveying Emotional Information. Cogn Comput 1, 268–278 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-009-9017-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-009-9017-8