Skip to main content
Log in

Learning in the brain–computer interface: insights about degrees of freedom and degeneracy from a landscape model of motor learning

  • Review
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we examine the role of degrees of freedom and their degeneracy in learning in the brain–computer interface paradigm. Though the traditional notion of degrees of freedom in motor learning gave emphasis to muscle and joint activity, the broader concept of dimensions of behavior is relevant to brain–computer interface learning where there is no muscle activity. The role of degeneracy in the dimensions of brain activity is proposed to enhance learning through robustness to stability loss and adaptability in the search for new stable states. Principles for the application of augmented information for learning to coordinate and control the degenerate degrees of freedom in the brain–computer interface are outlined.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams JA (1971) A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Motor Behav 3:111–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Basar E (ed) (1990) Chaos in brain function. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

  • Beek PJ, Turvey MT, Schmidt RC (1992) Autonomous and nonautonomous dynamics of coordinated rhythmic movement. Ecol Psychol 4:65–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilodeau EA, Bilodeau IM (1961) Motor skills learning. Ann R Psychol 12:243–280

    Google Scholar 

  • Birbaumer N, Ghanayim N, Hinterberger T, Iversen I, Kotchoubey B, Kübler A, Perelmouter J, Taub E, Flor H (1999) A spelling device for the paralysed. Nature 398:297–298

    Google Scholar 

  • Birbaumer N, Kübler A, Ghanayim N, Hinterberger T, Perelmouter J, Kaiser J, Iversen I, Kotchoubey B, Neumann N, Flor H (2000) The thought translation device (TTD) for completely paralyzed patients. IEEE Rehabil Eng 8:190–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Birbaumer N, Hinterberer T, Kübler A, Neumann N (2003) The thought-translation device (TTD): neurobehavioral mechanisms and clinical outcome. IEEE Neurol Sci 11:120–123

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen H-H, Liu Y-T, Mayer-Kress G, Newell KM (2005) Learning the pedalo locomotion task. J Motor Behav (in press)

  • Eckhardt RB (2000) Human paleobiology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman GM, Gally J (2001) Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:13763–13768

    Google Scholar 

  • Eubank SG, Farmer JD (1997) Probability, random processes, and the statistical description of dynamics. In: Lam L (ed) Introduction to nonlinear physics. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 106–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández P, Solé RV (2004) The role of computation in complex regulatory networks. In: Koonin E, Wolf Y, Karev G (eds) Scale-free networks and genome biology. Landes Bioscience, Georgetown

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler CA, Turvey MT (1978) Skill acquisition: an event approach with special reference to searching for the optimum of a function of several variables. In: Stelmach GE (ed) Information processing in motor control and learning. Academic, New York, pp 1–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (2003) The wave packet: an action potential for the 21st century. J Integr Neurosci 2:3–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ, Gaal G, Jorsten R (2003) A neurobiological theory of meaning in perception. Part III: multiple cortical areas synchronize without loss of local autonomy. Int J Bifurcation Chaos 13:2845–2856

    Google Scholar 

  • Friston KJ, Price CJ (2003) Degeneracy and redundancy in cognitive anatomy. Trends Cog Sci 7:151–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene PH (1969) Seeking mathematical models for skilled actions. In: Bootzin D, Muffley HC (eds) Biomechanics. Proceedings of the first Rock Island arsenal biomechanics symposium. Plenum, New York

  • Haken H (1996) Principles of brain functioning: a synergetic approach to brain activity, behavior and cognition. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1997) The cognitive neuroscience of action. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jen E (ed) (2002) Robust design: a repertoire of biological, ecological, and engineering case studies. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan MI (1990) Motor learning and the degrees of freedom problem. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance XIII. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 796–836

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay BA (1988) The dimensionality of movement trajectories and the degrees of freedom problem: a tutorial. Hum Move 7:343–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (1996) The Bernstein problem: how does the central nervous system make its choices? In: Latash ML, Turvey MT (eds) Dexterity and its development. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 277–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL (1992) Loss of ‘complexity’ and aging: potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA 267:1806–1809

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu YT, Mayer-Kress G, Newell KM (2004a) Beyond curve fitting to inferences about learning. J Motor Behav 36:233–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer-Kress G (1994) Localized measures for non-stationary time-series of physiological data. Integrative Physiol Behav Sci 29:205–210

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarland DJ, Wolpaw JR (2003) EEG-based communication and control: speed-accuracy relationships. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 28:217–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuper C, Muller GR, Kübler A, Birbaumer N, Pfurtscheller G (2003) Clinical application of an EEG-based brain-computer interface: a case study in a patient with severe motor impairment. Clin Neurol 114:399–409

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM (1976) Knowledge of results and motor learning. In: Keogh J, Hutton RS (eds) Exercise and sport sciences reviews, vol 4. Journal Publishing, Santa Barbara

  • Newell KM (1991) Motor skill acquisition. In: Rosenzweig MR, Porter LW (eds) Annual review of psychology, vol 42. Annual Reviews, Palo Alto, pp 213–237

  • Newell KM (1996a) Change in movement and skill: learning, retention, and transfer. In: Latash M, Turvey M (eds) Dexterity and its development. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 393–432

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM (1996b) The dynamics of stereotypic behaviors. In: Sprague RL, Newell KM (eds) Stereotypies: brain–behavior relationships. American Psychological Association, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, McDonald PV (1994) Learning to coordinate redundant biomechanical degrees of freedom. In: Swinnen S, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P (eds) Interlimb coordination: neural, dynamical and cognitive constraints. Academic, New York, pp 515–536

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, McGinnis PM (1985) Kinematic information feedback for skilled performance. Hum Learn 4:39–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Vaillancourt D (2001) Dimensional change in motor learning. Hum Move 4-5:695–716

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Valvano J (1998) Therapeutic intervention as a constraint in learning and relearning movement skills. Sci J Occup Ther 5:51–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Morris LR, Scully DM (1985) Augmented information and the acquisition of skill in physical activity. In: Terjung RL (ed) Exercise and sport sciences reviews, vol 13. Collamore, Lexington, pp 235–261

  • Newell KM, van Emmerik REA, McDonald PV (1989a) On simple movements and complex theories (and vice-versa). Behav Brain 12:229–230

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Kugler PN, van Emmerik REA, McDonald PV (1989b) Search strategies and the acquisition of coordination. In: Wallace SA (ed) Perspectives on coordination. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 86–122

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Liu YT, Mayer-Kress G (2001) Time scales in motor learning and development. Psychol Rev 108:57–82

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Liu Y-T, Mayer-Kress G (2003) A dynamical systems interpretation of epigenetic landscapes for infant motor development. Infant Behav 26:449–472

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez E, George N, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Renault B, Varela FJ (1999) Perception’s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 397:430–433

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle G (1949) The concept of mind. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

  • Saltzman EL (1979) Levels of sensorimotor representation. J Math Psychol 20:91–163

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaal S, Sternad D, Osu R, Kawato M (2004) Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete. Nat Neurosci 7:1136–1143

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schöner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snoddy GS (1926) Learning and stability. J Appl Psychol 10:1–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Sporns O, Edelman GM (1993) Solving Bernstein’s problem: a proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. Child Dev 64:960–981

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike EL (1913) Educational psychology, vol. 2. Columbia University, New York

  • Tononi G, Sporns O, Edelman GM (1999) Measures of degeneracy and redundancy in biological networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:3257–3262

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Shaw RE (1999) Ecological foundations of cognition: degrees of freedom and conserved quantities in animal-environment systems. J Conscious Stud 6:111–123

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Shaw RE, Mace W (1978) Issues in a theory of action: degrees of freedom, coordinative structures, and coalitions. In: Requin J (ed) Attention and performance VII. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 557–598

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker MP, Brakefield T, Hobson TA, Stickgold R (2003) Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425:616–620

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpaw JR, McFarland DJ (2004) Control of a two-dimensional movement signal by a noninvasive brain-computer interface in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:17849–17854

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpaw JR, Birbaumer N, Heetderks WJ, McFarland DJ, Peckham PH, Schalk G, Donchin E, Quatrano LA, Robinson CJ, Vaughan TM (2000) Brain-computer interface technology: a review of the first international meeting. IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng 8:164–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zatsiorsky VM (1998) Kinematics of human motion. Human Kinetics, Champaign

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karl M. Newell.

Additional information

Communicated by Irene Ruspantini and Niels Birbaumer

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Newell, K.M., Liu, YT. & Mayer-Kress, G. Learning in the brain–computer interface: insights about degrees of freedom and degeneracy from a landscape model of motor learning. Cogn Process 6, 37–47 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0047-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0047-6

Keywords

Navigation