Skip to main content
Log in

Thinking in action: thought made visible in contemporary dance

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Contemporary dance—movement deliberately and systematically cultivated for its own sake—is examined in the light of the procedural and declarative view of long-term knowledge. We begin with a description of two settings in which new works of contemporary dance are created and performed. Although non-verbal, contemporary dance can be a language declared through movement and stillness of the body. Ideas for new movement material come from objects, events or imaginings that are spoken, seen, heard, imagined, or felt. Declared through movement, the idea becomes visible. Communication in dance involves general psychological processes such as direct visual perception of motion and force, motor simulation via mirror neurons, and implicit learning of movement vocabularies and grammars. Creating and performing dance appear to involve both procedural and declarative knowledge. The latter includes the role of episodic memory in performance and occasional labelling of movement phrases and sections in rehearsal. Procedural knowledge in dance is augmented by expressive nuance, feeling and communicative intent that is not characteristic of other movement-based procedural tasks. Having delineated lexical and grammatical components in dance, neural mechanisms are identified based on Ullman’s (Ullman in Cognition 92:231–270, 2004) alignment of lexical knowledge with declarative memory and mental grammar with procedural memory. We conclude with suggestions for experiments to test these assumptions that concern thought in action in composition, performance and appreciation of contemporary dance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to note that dance and music performed separately from each other and observed passively is a largely Western and relatively recent phenomenon (Cross 2003). The mechanisms discussed have been informed by our analysis of contemporary dance in a Western setting. As perceptual mechanisms they are likely to apply to a range of stylistic and cultural contexts.

  2. According to the New Shorter Oxford English Dictionary (Brown 1993) language can refer to “a non verbal method of human communication, as gesture or facial expression, hand-signing, etc.; as a means of artistic expression, as dance, music, or painting”.

  3. Speculating, there is an intriguing possibility that a rule common to all forms of dance is for movement to be biomechanically possible. Experiments have demonstrated that biomechanically impossible motion reduces, for example, accurate pursuit of two-dimensional targets (Viviani et al. 1987), and recognition of emotion in point-light displays (Dittrich et al. 1996). Further experiments are needed to determine whether this is an example of slow, ungrammatical sequence learning, whether it relates to the role of acquired motor skills in action perception, or whether these two postulates are both correct and linked

References

  • Anderson JR (1983) The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR (2005) Cognitive psychology and its implications, 6th edn. Worth, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bharucha JJ (1994) Tonality and expectation. In: Aiello R (ed) Musical perceptions. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 213–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks LR, Vokey JR (1991) Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: a comment on Reber and Mathews et al. J Exp Psychol Gen 120:316–323

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown L (ed) (1993) The new shorter Oxford English dictionary. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grezes J, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2004) Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cort. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhi007

  • Castiello U (2003) Understanding other people’s actions: intention and attention. J Exp Psychol P 29:416–430

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaffin R, Imreh G, Crawford M (2002) Practicing perfection: memory and piano performance. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis MC (2003) From hand to mouth: the origins of language. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross I (2003) Music as biocultural phenomenon. In: Avanzini G, Faienza C, Minciacchi D et al (eds) The neurosciences and music. Ann NY Acad Sci 999:106–111

  • Crowder RG (1993) System and principles in memory theory: another critique of pure memory. In: Collins AF, Gathercole SE, Conway MA, Morris PE (eds) Theories of memory. Erlbaum, Hove, pp 139–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham M (1968) Changes: notes on choreography. Something Else Press Inc, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Heinemann, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittrich WH, Troscianko T, Lea SEG, Morgan D (1996) Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception 25:727–738

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev 100:363–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga L et al (1995) Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J Neurophys 73:2608–2611

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM, Posner MI (1967) Human performance. Brooks Cole, Belmont

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster R (1976) Knowing in my bones. Adam & Charles Black, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster SL (1986) Reading dancing: bodies and subjects in contemporary American dance. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V et al (1999) Perception through action. Psyche 5(21) http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v5/psyche-5-21-gallese.html

  • Gardner H (1993) Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences, 2nd edn. Harper Collins, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass R (2005) Observer response to contemporary dance. In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham M (1973) The notebooks of Martha Graham. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Grove R (1999) In the house of breathings. In: Graham S (ed) Papers and proceedings of Australia New Zealand Dance Research Society Forum, Auckland University of Technology, October

  • Grove R (2005) Show me what you just did. In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) (2005) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagendoorn I (2004) Some speculative hypotheses about the nature and perception of dance and choreography. J Consci St 11:79–110

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanna JL (1979) To dance is human: a theory of nonverbal communication. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Healey S (2005a) Quantum leapers. Documentary DVD

  • Healey S (2005b) Navigating fine lines. In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey D (1959) The art of making dances. Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer SK (1953) Feeling and form. Routledge & Kegan Paul Limited, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee K-M, Kim C-M, Woo S-H (2001) fMRI comparison between expert and novice perception of dance (Abstract). NeuroImage 6:907

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limón J (1955) Composing a dance. Juilliard Rev II:17–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch S (2005) Why do we like to dance and sing? In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

  • McKechnie S (1984) The search for meaning. In: Harwood L, Wilkinson H (eds) The journal of dance education, vol 1. Victorian Association for Dance Education in Schools, Melbourne

  • McKechnie S (1999) Unspoken knowledge: thinking in space and time. In: Papers and proceedings of the conference of the Asian Pacific confederation for arts education (ASPACAE), Lasalle-SIA, Singapore

  • McKechnie S (2001) Mind in motion: seeking a theory of choreographic cognition. Choreography Dance 6:139–154

    Google Scholar 

  • McKechnie S (2002) Movement as metaphor: the construction of meaning in the choreographic art. In: Stevens C, Burnham D, McPherson G, Schubert E, Renwick J (eds) Proceedings of the 7th international conference on music perception & cognition (ICMPC7), Causal Productions, Adelaide

  • McKechnie S (2004) From grands changements to grand narratives. In: Proceedings of dance rebooted conference, Deakin University

  • McKechnie S (2005) Dancing memes, minds and designs. In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • McKechnie S, Grove R (2000) Thinking bodies. Brolga: An Australian Journal About Dance 7–14

  • Meyer RK, Palmer C (2003) Temporal and motor transfer in music performance. Music Percept 21:81–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murata A et al (1997) Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. J Neurophys 78:2226–2230

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer C, Pfordresher PQ (2003) Incremental planning in sequence production. Psychol Rev 110:683–712

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack RA, Packard MG (2003) Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsycho 41:245–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber AS (1967) Implicit learning of artificial grammars. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 6:855–863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber AS (1993) Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert E (2001) Continuous measurement of self-report emotional response to music. In: Juslin P, Sloboda J (eds) Music and emotion: theory and research. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 393–414

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder B (2000) Music and memory: an introduction. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Stell MF (ed) (2001) Eternity: stories from the emotional heart of Australia. National Museum of Australia, Canberra

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens C (2005) Trans-disciplinary approaches to research into creation, performance and appreciation of contemporary dance. In: Grove R, Stevens C, McKechnie S (eds) Thinking in four dimensions: creativity and cognition in contemporary dance. Melbourne University Press, Carlton

  • Stevens C, McKechnie S (2005) Minds and motion: dynamical systems in choreography, creativity, and dance. In: Birringer J, Fenger J (eds) Tanz im Kopf: Yearbook 15 of the German Dance Research Society, 2004 for the section “Dance and Science”

  • Stevens K, McKechnie S, Malloch S, Petocz A (2000) Choreographic cognition: composing time and space. In: Woods C, Luck GB, Brochard R, Seddon F, O’Neill S, Sloboda JA (eds) Proceedings of the 6th international conference on music perception and cognition, University of Keele, Keele

  • Stevens C, Malloch S, McKechnie S (2001) Moving mind: the cognitive psychology of contemporary dance. Brolga: An Australian Journal About Dance, Ausdance, Canberra, pp 55–67

  • Stevens C, Malloch S, Haszard Morris R, McKechnie S (2002) Shaped time: a dynamical systems analysis of contemporary dance. In: Stevens C, Burnham D, McPherson G, Schubert E, Renwick J (eds) Proceedings of the 7th international conference on music perception and cognition. Causal Productions, Adelaide, pp 161–164

  • Stevens C, Malloch S, McKechnie S, Steven N (2003) Choreographic cognition: the time-course and phenomenology of creating a dance. Pragmatics Cognit 11:299–329

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen E (1995) Time-scale dynamics and the development of an embodied cognition. In: Port RF, van Gelder T (eds) Mind as motion: exploration of the dynamics of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 69–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman MT (2004) Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92:231–270

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman MT, Pierpont EI (2005) Specific language impairment is not specific to language: the procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex 41:399–433

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan D (1990) Merce Cunningham. In: Cage, Cunningham, Johns: dancers on a plane. Thames & Hudson, London, pp 81–87

  • Viviani P, Campadelli P, Mounoud P (1987) Visuo-manual pursuit tracking of human two-dimensional movements. J Exp Psychol P 13:62–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Australian Research Council through its Strategic Partnerships with Industry Research & Training (SPIRT) and Linkage research grant schemes (Grant Nos. C59930500 and LP0211991), MARCS Auditory Laboratories at the University of Western Sydney, the School of Dance at the Victorian College of the Arts, and industry partners the Australia Council, Ausdance, and the Australian Choreographic Centre. Our heartfelt thanks to the team at the Australian Choreographic Centre—Mark Gordon, Ruth Osborne, the Quantum Leap Choreographic Youth Ensemble, Eternity choreographers and composers, and film-maker Sue Healey; to Agnes Petocz, Shona Erskine, and John Sutton for discussions about art, thinking, and memory; and to Anna Smith, Stephen Malloch, and Rua Haszard Morris for assistance with motion tracking and analysis. Further information: http://www.ausdance.org.au/unspoken, http://www.ausdance.org.au/connections, http://marcs.uws.edu.au. Email: kj.stevens@uws.edu.au

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Catherine Stevens.

Additional information

Communicated by John Sutton

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stevens, C., McKechnie, S. Thinking in action: thought made visible in contemporary dance. Cogn Process 6, 243–252 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-005-0014-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-005-0014-x

Keywords

Navigation