Skip to main content
Log in

What if versus probabilistic scenarios: a neuroscientific analysis

  • S.I.: Recent Developments in Financial Modeling and Risk Management
  • Published:
Annals of Operations Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Nowadays financial products are extremely complex and the decision to choose among them could represent a stressful event for individuals. Information related to the risk/return profile of an investment instrument and the way it is represented (framing effect) are crucial in determining the outcome of individuals’ decisions. In this paper we consider two schemes that can be employed to represent the random performances of risky financial products, namely the what if and probabilistic scenarios frames. This paper aims at measuring the impact of the two above mentioned schemes on investors’ decision accuracy and peripheral nervous system activity. In particular, the goal is twofold: (1) to investigate the behavioural and physiological indexes elicited in the decision-making process of investors who have to choose on the basis of the two different schemes; (2) to investigate on the effect of time pressure when probabilistic scenarios or what if frames are used. The first point is investigated by means of a decision making task, while the second one by means of a perceptual one.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baradell, J. G., & Klein, K. (1993). Relationship of life stress and body consciousness to hypervigilant decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 267–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 190, 80–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M., Mullainathan, S., Karlan, D., Shafir, E., & Zinman, J. (2010). What’s advertising content worth? Evidence from a consumer credit marketing field experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125, 263–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castellano, R., & Cerqueti, R. (2013). Roots and effects of financial misperception in a stochastic dominance framework. Quality & Quantity, 47, 3371–3389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castellano, R., & Cerqueti, R. (2016). A theory of misperception in a stochastic dominance framework and its application to structured financial products. IMA Journal of Management Mathematics, 29, 23–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • CESR. (2010). Guidelines-Selection and presentation of performance scenarios in the key investor information document (KID) for structured UCITS. CESR/10-1318.

  • Christopoulos, G. I., Uy, M. A., & Yap, W. J. (2016). The body and the brain: measuring skin conductance responses to understand the emotional experience. Organizational Research Methods, 22, 394–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, A. D., Cox, D., & Zimet, G. (2006). Understanding consumer responses to product risk information. Journal of Marketing, 70, 79–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Courtney, C. G. (2011). The skin conductance response, anticipation, and decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4, 111–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Goeij, P., Hogendoorn, T., & Campenhout, G.V. (2014). Pictures are worth a thousand words: Graphical information and investment decision making, Mimeo.

  • Fischer, G. W. (1989). Prescriptive decision science: Problems and opportunities. Annals of Operations Research, 19, 489–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frydman, C., & Camerer, C. F. (2016). The psychology and neuroscience of financial decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(9), 661–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. R. (1999). A bias toward short-term thinking in threat-related negative emotional states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 65–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, A., & Murray, W. (2003). How to spend and invest retirement savings. Annals of Operations Research, 124, 205–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hämäläinen, R. P., Luoma, J., & Saarinen, E. (2013). On the importance of behavioural operational research: The case of understanding and communicating about dynamic systems. European Journal of Operational Research, 228, 623–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillenbrand, A., & Schmelzer, A. (2017). Beyond information: Disclosure, distracted attention, and investor behavior. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 16, 14–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, J. C., & White, A. (1990). Pricing interest-rate derivative securities. Review of Financial Studies, 3, 573–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, J., & White, A. (1994). Numerical procedures for implementing term structure models I: Single-factor models. Journal of Derivatives, 2, 7–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, J., & White, A. (1996). Using Hull–White interest rate trees. Journal of Derivatives, 3, 26–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lajante, M., Droulers, O., Dondaine, T., & Amarantini, D. (2012). Opening the “black box” of electrodermal activity in consumer neuroscience research. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 5, 238–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liuzza, M. T., Olofsson, J. K., Sabiniewicz, A., & Sorokowska, A. (2017). Body odor trait disgust sensitivity predicts perception of sweat biosamples. Chemical Senses, 42, 479–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelić, A., Tieri, G., De Matteis, F., Babiloni, F., & Vecchiato, G. (2016). The enactive approach to architectural experience: A neurophysiological perspective on embodiment, motivation, and affordances. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrow, R. A., & Turnbull, S. M. (1995). Pricing derivatives on financial securities subject to credit risk. The Journal of Finance, 1, 53–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. (2007). Financial literacy and retirement preparedness. Evidence and implications for financial education. Business Economics, 42, 35–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S. U., Makwana, A. B., & Hare, T. A. (2015). Acute stress impairs self-control in goal-directed choice by altering multiple functional connections within the Brain’s decision circuits. Neuron, 87, 622–632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minenna, M. (2011). A quantitative framework to assess the risk-reward profile of non-equity products. London: Risk Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pabst, S., Brand, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2013). Stress and decision making: A few minutes make all the difference. Behavioural Brain Research, 250, 39–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palomaki, J., Kosunen, I., Kuikkaniemi, K., Yamabe, T., & Ravaja, N. (2013). Anticipatory electrodermal activity and decision making in a computer poker-game. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 6, 55–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panasiti, M. S., Ponsi, G., Monachesi, B., Lorenzini, L., Panasiti, V., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Cognitive load and emotional processing in psoriasis: A thermal imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 237, 211–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponsi, G., Monachesi, B., Panasiti, V., Aglioti, S. M., & Panasiti, M. S. (2019). Physiological and behavioral reactivity to social exclusion: A functional infrared thermal imaging study in patients with psoriasis. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121, 38–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponsi, G., Panasiti, M. S., Aglioti, S. M., & Liuzza, M. T. (2017a). Right-wing authoritarianism and stereotype-driven expectations interact in shaping intergroup trust in one-shot versus multiple-round social interactions. PLoS ONE, 12, e0190142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponsi, G., Panasiti, M. S., Rizza, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2017b). Thermal facial reactivity patterns predict social categorization bias triggered by unconscious and conscious emotional stimuli. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284, 18–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponsi, G., Panasiti, M. S., Scandola, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2016). Influence of warmth and competence on the promotion of safe in-group selection: Stereotype content model and social categorization of faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1464–1479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2009). Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychological Science, 20, 278–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2017). Stress and decision making: effects on valuation, learning, and risk-taking. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 14, 33–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, S. (2016). What-if analysis. In L. Liu & T. Özsu (Eds.), Encyclopedia of database systems (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starcke, K., & Brand, M. (2012). Decision making under stress: A selective review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1228–1248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tieri, G., Morone, G., Paolucci, S., & Iosa, M. (2018). Virtual reality in cognitive and motor rehabilitation: Facts, fiction and fallacies. Expert Review of Medical Devices, 15, 107–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tieri, G., Tidoni, E., Pavone, E. F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2015). Body visual discontinuity affects feeling of ownership and skin conductance responses. Scientific Reports, 5, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of decisions. The Journal of Business, 59, 251–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vecchiato, G., Jelic, A., Tieri, G., Maglione, A. G., De Matteis, F., & Babiloni, F. (2015a). Neurophysiological correlates of embodiment and motivational factors during the perception of virtual architectural environments. Cognitive Processing, 16, 425–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vecchiato, G., Tieri, G., Jelic, A., Maglione, A. G., De Matteis, F., & Babiloni, F. (2015b). Electroencephalographic correlates of sensorimotor integration and embodiment during the appreciation of virtual architectural environments. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 425–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vlaev, I., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2008). Risk preference discrepancy: A prospect relativity account of the discrepancy between risk preferences in laboratory gambles and real world investments. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 9, 132–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A. (2009). Interplay of investors’ financial knowledge and risk taking. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 10, 204–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, R. (2016). Stress potentiates decision biases: A stress induced deliberation-to-intuition (SIDI) model. Neurobiology of Stress, 3, 83–95.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rosella Castellano.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Castellano, R., Mancinelli, M., Ponsi, G. et al. What if versus probabilistic scenarios: a neuroscientific analysis. Ann Oper Res 299, 331–347 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10479-019-03272-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10479-019-03272-5

Keywords

Navigation