Abstract
We investigated the emergence of orienting behavior in artificial organisms that evolved following a genetic algorithm. These organisms live in a simulated environment containing food and danger elements and reproduce selectively based on the capacity of each individual to eat food while avoiding danger. When the amount of computational resources (number of hidden units) is adequate to the difficulty of the perceptual discrimination between food and danger, peripheral vision is sufficient to trigger stimulus identification. When the resources are scarce, the central portion of the sensory surface becomes a ‘fovea’, and the presence of a stimulus in peripheral vision triggers an orienting movement (foveation), before the organism can decide whether to eat or to avoid the object. Thus, orienting movements, as well as the segregation of processing resources into a high-definition fovea and a poor-definition periphery, may originate from a disproportion between complex perceptual tasks and (relatively) scarce computational resources.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Sokolov, E. N.: Higher nervous functions: the orienting reflex, Annu. Rev. Physiol., 25 (1963), 545–580.
Posner, M. I.: Orienting of attention, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 32 (1980), 3–25.
Yantis. S.: Attentional capture in vision, In: A. F. Kramer, G. H. Coles, G. D. Logan, (eds.) Converging Operations in the Study of Visual Selective Attention, pp. 45–76, American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, 1995.
Gainotti, G., D'Erme, P. and Bartolomeo, P.: Early orientation of attention toward the half space ipsilateral to the lesion in patients with unilateral brain damage, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 54 (1991), 1082–1089.
Bartolomeo, P.: The novelty effect in recovered hemineglect, Cortex, 33 (1997), 323–332.
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J. and Rafal, R. D.: Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention, J. Neurosci., 4 (1984), 1863–1874.
Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., Pook, P. K. and Rao, R. P. N.: Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition, Behav. Brain Sci., 20 (1997), 723–767..
Ballard, D. H.: Animate vision, Artificial Intelligence, 48 (1991), 57–86.
O'Regan, J. K.: Solving the 'real' mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46 (1992), 461–488.
Kosslyn, S. M. and Koenig, O.: Wet Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience. The Free Press: New York, 1992.
Parasuraman, R.: The attentive brain: issues and prospects, In: R. Parasuraman, (ed.) The Attentive Brain, pp. 3–15, The MIT Press: Cambridge (MA), 1998.
Parisi, D., Cecconi, F. and Nolfi, S.: Econets: Neural networks that learn in an environment, Network, 1 (1990), 149–168.
Rumelhart, D. E. and McClelland, J. L.: Parallel Distributed Processing. Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. 1986, 000–000.
Holland, J. J.: Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems. University of Michigan Press: Ann Arbor, Michigan, 1975.
Mitchell, M.: An Introduction to Genetic Algorithms. The MIT Press: Cambridge (MA), 1997.
Allport, D. A.: Visual attention, In: M. I. Posner, (ed.) Foundations of Cognitive Science, MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, 1989, pp. 631–687.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Bartolomeo, P., Pagliarini, L. & Parisi, D. Emergence of Orienting Behavior in Ecological Neural Networks. Neural Processing Letters 15, 69–76 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013896827975
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013896827975