Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton May 26, 2019

Subphonemic Variation and Lexical Processing: Social and Stylistic Factors

  • Zack Jones and Cynthia G. Clopper
From the journal Phonetica

Abstract

Different pronunciation variants of the same word can facilitate lexical access, but they may be more or less effective primes depending on their phonological form, stylistic appropriateness, familiarity, and social prestige, suggesting that multiple phonological variants are encoded in the lexicon with varying strength. The current study investigated how subphonemic variation is encoded using a lexical decision task with cross-modal form priming. The results revealed that the magnitude of priming was mediated by stylistic and social properties of the auditory primes, including speaking style, talker dialect, and duration. These interactions provide evidence that phonetically reduced forms and forms that are not socially prestigious are not as robustly encoded in the lexicon as canonical forms and forms produced in prestigious varieties.


verified



*Zack Jones, Oxley Hall 100, 1712 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210 (USA), E-Mail jones.5028@osu.edu

References

1 Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage.J Exp Psychol: Human, 10(3), 340357. 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.3400096-1523Search in Google Scholar PubMed

2 Bard, E. G., Anderson, A. H., Sotillo, C., Aylett, M., Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Newlands, A. (2000). Controlling the intelligibility of referring expressions in dialogue.Journal of Memory and Language, 42(1), 122. 10.1006/jmla.1999.26670749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

3 Becker, C. A. (1979). Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition.J Exp Psychol: Human, 5(2), 252259. 10.1037/0096-1523.5.2.2520096-1523Search in Google Scholar PubMed

4 Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics.Speech Communication, 20(3), 255272. 10.1016/S0167-6393(96)00063-50167-6393Search in Google Scholar PubMed

5 Burdin, R. S., Turnbull, R., & Clopper, C. G. (2015). Interactions among lexical and discourse characteristics in vowel production.Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Acoustical Society of America, 22(060005), 118. 10.1121/2.00000841939-800XSearch in Google Scholar

6 Clopper, C. G., & Bradlow, A. R. (2008). Perception of dialect variation in noise: Intelligibility and classification.Language and Speech, 51(Pt 3), 175198. 10.1177/00238309080985390023-8309Search in Google Scholar PubMed

7 Clopper, C. G., & Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2008). Effects of semantic predictability and regional dialect on vowel space reduction.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(3), 16821688. 10.1121/1.29533220001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

8 Clopper, C. G., Pierrehumbert, J. B., & Tamati, T. N. (2010). Lexical neighborhoods and phonological confusability in cross-dialect word recognition in noise.Laboratory Phonology, 1(1), 6592. 10.1515/labphon.2010.0051868-6346Search in Google Scholar

9 Clopper, C. G., Tamati, T. N., & Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2016). Variation in the strength of lexical encoding across dialects.Journal of Phonetics, 58, 87103. 10.1016/j.wocn.2016.06.0020095-4470Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10 Clopper, C. G., & Walker, A. (2017). Effects of lexical competition and dialect exposure on phonological priming.Language and Speech, 60(1), 85109. 10.1177/00238309166437370023-8309Search in Google Scholar PubMed

11 Colombo, L. (1986). Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words.J Exp Psychol: Human, 12(2), 226234. 10.1037/0096-1523.12.2.226Search in Google Scholar

12 Connine, C. M., Ranbom, L. J., & Patterson, D. J. (2008). Processing variant forms in spoken word recognition: The role of variant frequency.Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 403411. 10.3758/PP.70.3.4030031-5117Search in Google Scholar PubMed

13 Dufour, S., Nguyen, N., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2007). The perception of phonemic contrasts in a non-native dialect.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(4), EL131EL136. 10.1121/1.27107420001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

14 ErnestusM, BaayenRH (2007): The comprehension of acoustically reduced morphologically complex words: The roles of deletion, duration, and frequency of occurrence.Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, pp 773-776.Search in Google Scholar

15 Fowler, C. A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers’ signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction.Journal of Memory and Language, 26(5), 489504. 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90136-70749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

16 Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access.Psychological Review, 105(2), 251279. 10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.2510033-295XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

17 Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition.Journal of Memory and Language, 28(5), 501518. 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90009-00749-596XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

18 Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Marcario, J. K. (1992). Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: The roles of competition and bias.J Exp Psychol: Learn, 18(6), 12111238. 10.1037/0278-7393.18.6.12110278-7393Search in Google Scholar PubMed

19 Gow, D. W., Jr. (2001). Assimilation and anticipation in continuous spoken word recognition.Journal of Memory and Language, 45(1), 133159. 10.1006/jmla.2000.27640749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

20 Hamburger, M., & Slowiaczek, L. M. (1996). Phonological priming reflects lexical competition.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 520525. 10.3758/BF032145581069-9384Search in Google Scholar PubMed

21 Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically similar words: The encoding-bias hypothesis reconsidered.Memory & Cognition, 8(2), 115123. 10.3758/BF032134140090-502XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

22 Janse, E., Nooteboom, S. G., & Quene, H. (2007). Coping with gradient forms of /t/-deletion and lexical ambiguity in spoken word recognition.Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(2), 161200. 10.1080/016909605003710240169-0965Search in Google Scholar

23 Johnson, K. (1997). Speech perception without speaker normalization: An exemplar model. In K.Johnson & J. W.Mullennix (Eds.),Talker Variability in Speech Processing (pp. 145165). San Diego: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

24 Johnson, K. (2006). Resonance in an exemplar-based lexicon: The emergence of social identity and phonology.Journal of Phonetics, 34(4), 485499. 10.1016/j.wocn.2005.08.0040095-4470Search in Google Scholar

25 Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Search in Google Scholar

26 Labov, W. (1990). The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change.Language Variation and Change, 2(02), 205254. 10.1017/S09543945000003380954-3945Search in Google Scholar

27 Labov, W., Ash, S., & Boberg, C. (2005). The Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, Phonology and Sound Change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110167467Search in Google Scholar

28 Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatical context on the production and perception of speech.Language and Speech, 6(3), 172187. 10.1177/0023830963006003060023-8309Search in Google Scholar

29 Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model.Ear and Hearing, 19(1), 136. 10.1097/00003446-199802000-000010196-0202Search in Google Scholar PubMed

30 McLennan, C. T., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (2003). Representation of lexical form.J Exp Psychol: Learn, 29(4), 539553. 10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.5390278-7393Search in Google Scholar PubMed

31 McLennan, C. T., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (2005). Representation of lexical form: Evidence from studies of sublexical ambiguity.J Exp Psychol: Human, 31(6), 13081314. 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.13080096-1523Search in Google Scholar PubMed

32 Nusbaum, H. C., Pisoni, D. B., & Davis, C. K. (1984). Sizing up the Hoosier Mental Lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on Speech Perception Progress Report, Speech Research Laboratory. Indiana University, 10, 357376.Search in Google Scholar

33 Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In J.Bybee & P.Hopper (Eds.),Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure (pp. 137157). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tsl.45.08pieSearch in Google Scholar

34 Pitt, M. A. (2009). How are pronunciation variants of spoken words recognized? A test of generalization to newly learned words.Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 1936. 10.1016/j.jml.2009.02.0050749-596XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

35 Radeau, M., Morais, J., & Dewier, A. (1989). Phonological priming in spoken word recognition: Task effects.Memory & Cognition, 17(5), 525535. 10.3758/BF031970740090-502XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

36 Radeau, M., Morais, J., & Segui, J. (1995). Phonological priming between monosyllabic spoken words.J Exp Psychol: Human, 21, 12971310.10.1037/0096-1523.21.6.1297Search in Google Scholar

37 Rae, M., & Warren, P. (2002). Goldilocks and the three bears: Sound merger and recognition in NZE.New Zealand English Journal, 16, 3341.Search in Google Scholar

38 Ranbom, L. J., & Connine, C. M. (2007). Lexical representation of phonological variation in spoken word recognition.Journal of Memory and Language, 57(2), 273298. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.04.0010749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

39 Ranbom, L. J., Connine, C. M., & Yudman, E. M. (2009). Is phonological context always used to recognize variant forms in spoken word recognition? The role of variant frequency and context distribution.J Exp Psychol: Human, 35(4), 12051220. 10.1037/a00150220096-1523Search in Google Scholar PubMed

40 Slowiaczek, L. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (1986). Effects of phonological similarity on priming in auditory lexical decision.Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 230237. 10.3758/BF031976980090-502XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

41 Sóskuthy, M. (2015). Understanding change through stability: A computational study of sound change actuation.Lingua, 163, 4060. 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.05.0100024-3841Search in Google Scholar

42 Sumner, M. (2013). A phonetic explanation of pronunciation variant effects.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(1), EL26EL32. 10.1121/1.48074320001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

43 Sumner, M., & Kataoka, R. (2013). Effects of phonetically-cued talker variation on semantic encoding.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(6), EL485EL491. 10.1121/1.48261510001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

44 Sumner, M., Kim, S. K., King, E., & McGowan, K. B. (2014). The socially weighted encoding of spoken words: A dual-route approach to speech perception.Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1015. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.010151664-1078Search in Google Scholar PubMed

45 Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2005). Perception and representation of regular variation: The case of word-final /t/.Journal of Memory and Language, 52(3), 330346. 10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.0040749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

46 Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants.Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4), 487501. 10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.0010749-596XSearch in Google Scholar

47 Tucker, B. V. (2011). The effect of reduction on the processing of flaps and /g/ in isolated words.Journal of Phonetics, 39(3), 312318. 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.0010095-4470Search in Google Scholar

48 TuckerBV, WarnerN (2007): Inhibition of processing due to the reduction of the American English flap.Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, pp 773-776.Search in Google Scholar

49 van de Ven, M., Tucker, B. V., & Ernestus, M. (2011). Semantic context effects in the comprehension of reduced pronunciation variants.Memory & Cognition, 39(7), 13011316. 10.3758/s13421-011-0103-20090-502XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

50 Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials.Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 380393. 10.3758/BF031971270090-502XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

51 Warren, P., Hay, J., & Thomas, B. (2007). The loci of sound change effects in recognition and perception. In J.Cole & J. I.Hualde (Eds.),Laboratory Phonology 9 (pp. 87112). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

  1. 1

    An alternative interpretation of Ranbom and Connine’s (2007) results is that the different numbers of segments in the stimuli containing [nt] and [ɾ̃] variants led to the observed differences in lexical processing.

  2. 2

    In the original corpus, two disyllabic word targets were used to balance the set of words for lexical frequency and neighborhood density. To maintain this balance, all 234 target words were used in the current study.

  3. 3

    Hand dominance was not assessed because only correct word responses were analyzed. Any differences in response times to word responses as a function of hand dominance were captured by the random intercepts for participants in the statistical analysis (see the section “Statistical Analysis”).

  4. 4

    Although other factors, including intrinsic vowel duration differences and phonetic context, could also play a role in vowel duration variation, these word-specific effects are captured by the random intercepts and trial type slopes for prime word.

Received: 2017-04-03
Accepted: 2018-09-21
Published Online: 2019-05-26
Published in Print: 2019-05-01

© 2019 S. Karger AG, Basel

Downloaded on 18.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1159/000493982/html
Scroll to top button