Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton April 24, 2019

Language Specificity in Phonetic Cue Weighting: Monolingual and Bilingual Perception of the Stop Voicing Contrast in English and Spanish

  • Jessamyn Schertz ORCID logo , Kathy Carbonell and Andrew J. Lotto
From the journal Phonetica

Abstract

Background/Aims: This work examines the perception of the stop voicing contrast in Spanish and English along four acoustic dimensions, comparing monolingual and bilingual listeners. Our primary goals are to test the extent to which cue-weighting strategies are language-specific in monolinguals, and whether this language specificity extends to bilingual listeners. Methods: Participants categorized sounds varying in voice onset time (VOT, the primary cue to the contrast) and three secondary cues: fundamental frequency at vowel onset, first formant (F1) onset frequency, and stop closure duration. Listeners heard acoustically identical target stimuli, within language-specific carrier phrases, in English and Spanish modes. Results: While all listener groups used all cues, monolingual English listeners relied more on F1, and less on closure duration, than monolingual Spanish listeners, indicating language specificity in cue use. Early bilingual listeners used the three secondary cues similarly in English and Spanish, despite showing language-specific VOT boundaries. Conclusion: While our findings reinforce previous work demonstrating language-specific phonetic representations in bilinguals in terms of VOT boundary, they suggest that this specificity may not extend straightforwardly to cue-weighting strategies.


verified



*Jessamyn Schertz, Department of Language Studies, University of Toronto Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6 (Canada), E-Mail jessamyn.schertz@utoronto.ca

References

1 Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1985). Relative power of cues: F0 shift versus voice timing. In Fromkin, V. (Ed.), Phonetic linguistics: Essays in honor of Peter Ladefoged (pp. 25–33).Search in Google Scholar

2 Antoniou, M., Tyler, M. D., & Best, C. T. (2012). Two ways to listen: Do L2-dominant bilinguals perceive stop voicing according to language mode?Journal of Phonetics, 40(4), 582594. 10.1016/j.wocn.2012.05.0050095-4470Search in Google Scholar PubMed

3 BenkíJ R (2005): Perception of VOT and first formant onset by Spanish and English speakers. in CohenJ, McAlisterK, RolstadK, MacSwanJ(eds), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, pp. 204248.Search in Google Scholar

4 BoersmaP, WeeninkD (2014): Praat: doing phonetics by computer, version 5.2.35, http://www.praat.orgSearch in Google Scholar

5 Bradlow, A. R. (1995). A comparative acoustic study of English and Spanish vowels.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(3), 19161924. 10.1121/1.4120640001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

6 Caramazza, A., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Zurif, E. B., & Carbone, E. (1973). The acquisition of a new phonological contrast: The case of stop consonants in French-English bilinguals.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54(2), 421428. 10.1121/1.19135940001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

7 Chodroff, E., & Wilson, C. (2017). Structure in talker-specific phonetic realization: Covariation of stop consonant VOT in American English.Journal of Phonetics, 61, 3047. 10.1016/j.wocn.2017.01.0010095-4470Search in Google Scholar

8 Clayards, M. (2018a). Individual talker and token covariation in the production of multiple cues to stop voicing.Phonetica, 75(1), 123. 10.1159/0004488090031-8388Search in Google Scholar PubMed

9 Clayards, M. (2018b). Differences in cue weights for speech perception are correlated for individuals within and across contrasts.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(3), EL172EL177. 10.1121/1.50520250001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10 Crystal, T. H., & House, A. S. (1988). The duration of American English stop consonants: An overview.Journal of Phonetics, 16(3), 285294.0095-447010.1016/S0095-4470(19)30503-0Search in Google Scholar

11 De Rosario-MartinezH (2015): Package ‘phia’, https://github.com/heliosdrm/phiaSearch in Google Scholar

12 DiehlR L, KluenderK R, WalshM A (1990): Some auditory bases of speech perception and production. Advances in speech, hearing and language processing 1:243–268.Search in Google Scholar

13 Dmitrieva, O., Llanos, F., Shultz, A. A., & Francis, A. L. (2015). Phonological status, not voice onset time, determines the acoustic realization of onset f0 as a secondary voicing cue in Spanish and English.Journal of Phonetics, 49, 7795. 10.1016/j.wocn.2014.12.0050095-4470Search in Google Scholar

14 Elman, J. L., Diehl, R., & Buchwald, S. E. (1977). Perceptual switching in bilinguals.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 62(4), 971974. 10.1121/1.3815910001-4966Search in Google Scholar

15 Escudero, P., Benders, T., & Lipski, S. (2009). Native, non-native and L2 perceptual cue weighting for Dutch vowels: The case of Dutch, German, and Spanish listeners.Journal of Phonetics, 37(4), 452465. 10.1016/j.wocn.2009.07.0060095-4470Search in Google Scholar

16 Francis, A. L., Kaganovich, N., & Driscoll-Huber, C. (2008). Cue-specific effects of categorization training on the relative weighting of acoustic cues to consonant voicing in English.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(2), 12341251. 10.1121/1.29451610001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

17 García-Sierra, A., Ramírez-Esparza, N., Silva-Pereyra, J., Siard, J., & Champlin, C. A. (2012). Assessing the double phonemic representation in bilingual speakers of Spanish and English: An electrophysiological study.Brain and Language, 121(3), 194205. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.03.0080093-934XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

18 Gonzales, K., & Lotto, A. J. (2013). A Bafri, un Pafri: Bilinguals’ Pseudoword identifications support language-specific phonetic systems.Psychological Science, 24(11), 21352142. 10.1177/09567976134864850956-7976Search in Google Scholar PubMed

19 Green, K. P., Zampini, M. L., & Magloire, J. (1997). An examination of word-initial-stop closure interval in English, Spanish, and Spanish-English bilinguals.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(5), 3136. 10.1121/1.4206480001-4966Search in Google Scholar

20 Haggard, M., Ambler, S., & Callow, M. (1970). Pitch as a voicing cue.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 47(2, 2B), 613617. 10.1121/1.19119360001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

21 Harris, J. (1969). Spanish Phonology. MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

22 Hazan, V., & Boulakia, G. (1993). Perception and production of a voicing contrast by French-English bilinguals.Language and Speech, 36(1), 1738. 10.1177/0023830993036001020023-8309Search in Google Scholar

23 Hazan, V., & Rosen, S. (1991). Individual variability in the perception of cues to place contrasts in initial stops.Perception & Psychophysics, 49(2), 187200. 10.3758/BF032050380031-5117Search in Google Scholar PubMed

24 Holt, L. L., & Lotto, A. J. (2006). Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second language acquisition.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5 Pt 1), 30593071. 10.1121/1.21883770001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

25 Holt, L. L., Lotto, A. J., & Kluender, K. R. (2001). Influence of fundamental frequency on stop-consonant voicing perception: A case of learned covariation or auditory enhancement?The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(2), 764774. 10.1121/1.13398250001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

26 Idemaru, K., Holt, L. L., & Seltman, H. (2012). Individual differences in cue weights are stable across time: The case of Japanese stop lengths.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(6), 39503964. 10.1121/1.47650760001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

27 Johnson, C. E., & Wilson, I. L. (2002). Phonetic evidence for early language differentiation: Research issues and some preliminary data.The International Journal of Bilingualism, 6(3), 271289. 10.1177/136700690200600304011367-0069Search in Google Scholar

28 Kapnoula, E. C., Winn, M. B., Kong, E. J., Edwards, J., & McMurray, B. (2017). Evaluating the sources and functions of gradiency in phoneme categorization: An individual differences approach.Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 43(9), 15941611. 10.1037/xhp00004100096-1523Search in Google Scholar PubMed

29 Kim, D., Clayards, M., & Goad, H. (2017). Individual differences in second language speech perception across tasks and contrasts: The case of English vowel contrasts by Korean learners.Linguistics Vanguard : Multimodal Online Journal, 3(1). 10.1515/lingvan-2016-00252199-174XSearch in Google Scholar

30 Kingston, J., & Diehl, R. L. (1994). Phonetic knowledge.Language, 70(3), 419454. 10.1353/lan.1994.00230097-8507Search in Google Scholar

31 Kingston, J., & Diehl, R. L. (1995). Intermediate properties in the perception of distinctive feature values.Papers in Laboratory Phonology, 4, 727. 10.1017/CBO9780511554315.002Search in Google Scholar

32 Kingston, J., Diehl, R. L., Kirk, C. J., & Castleman, W. A. (2008). On the internal perceptual structure of distinctive features: The [voice] contrast.Journal of Phonetics, 36(1), 2854. 10.1016/j.wocn.2007.02.0010095-4470Search in Google Scholar PubMed

33 Kluender, K. R. (1991). Effects of first formant onset properties on voicing judgments result from processes not specific to humans.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90(1), 8396. 10.1121/1.4022850001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

34 Kluender, K. R., & Lotto, A. J. (1994). Effects of first formant onset frequency on [-voice] judgments result from auditory processes not specific to humans.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(2), 10441052. 10.1121/1.4084660001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

35 Kong, E. J., & Yoon, I. (2013). L2 proficiency effect on the acoustic cue-weighting pattern by Korean L2 learners of English: Production and perception of English stops.Journal of the Korean Society of Speech Sciences, 5(4), 8190. 10.13064/KSSS.2013.5.4.081Search in Google Scholar

36 Lisker, L. (1957). Closure duration and the intervocalic voiced-voiceless distinction in English.Language, 33(1), 4249. 10.2307/4109490097-8507Search in Google Scholar

37 Lisker, L. (1986). “Voicing” in English: A catalogue of acoustic features signaling /b/ versus /p/ in trochees.Language and Speech, 29(Pt 1), 311. 10.1177/0023830986029001020023-8309Search in Google Scholar PubMed

38 Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements.Word, 20(3), 384422. 10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830Search in Google Scholar

39 Lisker, L., Liberman, A. M., Erickson, D. M., Dechovitz, D., & Mandler, R. (1977). On pushing the voice-onset-time (vot) boundary about.Language and Speech, 20(3), 209216. 10.1177/0023830977020003030023-8309Search in Google Scholar PubMed

40 Llanos, F., Dmitrieva, O., Shultz, A., & Francis, A. L. (2013). Auditory enhancement and second language experience in Spanish and English weighting of secondary voicing cues.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(3), 22132224. 10.1121/1.48178450001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

41 Löfqvist, A., Baer, T., McGarr, N. S., & Story, R. S. (1989). The cricothyroid muscle in voicing control.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85(3), 13141321. 10.1121/1.3974620001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

42 Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D. (2014). Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing.Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 89117. 10.1017/S13667289130000841366-7289Search in Google Scholar

43 Moulines, E., & Chapentier, F. (1990). Pitchsynchronous waveform processing techniques for text-to-speech synthesis using diphones.Speech Communication, 9(5-6), 453467. 10.1016/0167-6393(90)90021-Z0167-6393Search in Google Scholar

44 Nearey, T. M. (1997). Speech perception as pattern recognition.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101(6), 32413254. 10.1121/1.4182900001-4966Search in Google Scholar PubMed

45 Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python.Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 813. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.0170165-0270Search in Google Scholar PubMed

46 R Core Team (2013): R: A language and environment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, http://www.R-project.orgSearch in Google Scholar

47 SchertzJ (2014): The structure and plasticity of phonetic categories across languages and modalities. PhD thesis, University of Arizona.Search in Google Scholar

48 Schertz, J., Cho, T., Lotto, A., & Warner, N. (2015). Individual differences in phonetic cue use in production and perception of a non-native sound contrast.Journal of Phonetics, 52, 183204. 10.1016/j.wocn.2015.07.0030095-4470Search in Google Scholar PubMed

49 SimonetM, HualdeJ I, NadeuM (2012): Lenition of /d/ in spontaneous Spanish and Catalan. Proceedings of Interspeech.10.21437/Interspeech.2012-50Search in Google Scholar

50 Stilp, C. E., Rogers, T. T., & Kluender, K. R. (2010). Rapid efficient coding of correlated complex acoustic properties.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(50), 2191421919. 10.1073/pnas.10090201070027-8424Search in Google Scholar PubMed

51 Williams, L. (1977). The perception of stop consonant voicing by Spanish-English bilinguals.Perception & Psychophysics, 21(4), 289297. 10.3758/BF031994770031-5117Search in Google Scholar

52 Zampini, M., & Green, K. (2001). The voicing contrast in English and Spanish: The relationship between perception and production. In J.Nicol (Ed.),One Mind, Two Languages (pp. 2348). Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

  1. 1

    The baseline token was created by splicing aspiration from an English /pa/ onto the Spanish vowel. There are differences in quality between the nonfront low vowels in English and Spanish (Bradlow, 1995), and the Spanish vowel was chosen because it did not sound out of place in the English, while the English vowel did sound out of place in the Spanish, as judged by native speakers of each language.

  2. 2

    Selected stimuli are publicly available at the website of the first author; full set available upon request.

  3. 3

    glmer(choice ∼ language × (vot + f0 + F1 + closure) + (vot + f0 + F1 + closure | participant).

Received: 2018-07-25
Accepted: 2019-01-28
Published Online: 2019-04-24
Published in Print: 2020-04-01

© 2019 S. Karger AG, Basel

Downloaded on 4.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1159/000497278/html
Scroll to top button