skip to main content
10.1145/2388676.2388704acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication Pagesicmi-mlmiConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Effects of modality on virtual button motion and performance

Published:22 October 2012Publication History

ABSTRACT

The simple action of pressing a button is a multimodal interaction with an interesting depth of complexity. As the development of computer interfaces supporting 3D tasks progresses, there is a need to understand how users will interact with virtual buttons that generate multimodal feedback. Using a phone number dialing task on a virtual keypad, this study examined the effects of visual, auditory, and haptic feedback combinations on task performance and on the motion of individual button presses. The results suggest that the resistance of haptic feedback alone was not enough to prevent participants from pressing the button farther than necessary. Reinforcing haptic feedback with visual or auditory feedback shortened the depth of the presses significantly. However, the shallower presses that occurred with trimodal feedback may have led participants to release some buttons too early, which may explain an unexpected increase in mistakes where the participant missed digits from the phone number.

References

  1. E. M. Altinsoy. Perceptual aspects of auditory-tactile asynchrony. In Proc. ICSV 10, pages 3831--3838, 2003.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. J. L. Burke, M. S. Prewett, A. A. Gray, L. Yang, F. R. B. Stilson, M. D. Coovert, L. R. Elliot, and E. Redden. Comparing the effects of visual-auditory and visual-tactile feedback on user performance: a meta-analysis. In Proc. ICMI '06, pages 108--117. ACM, Nov. 2006. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  3. C. Clare. Human factors: A most important ingredient in keyboard designs. EDN Magazine, 21(8):99--102, 1976.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  4. K. M. Cohen. Membrane keyboards and human performance. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting Proceedings, 26:424--424, 1982.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  5. F. Colavita. Human sensory dominance. Perception and Psycophysics, 16(2):409--412, 1974.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  6. I. n. Díaz, J. Hernantes, I. Mansa, A. Lozano, D. Borro, J. J. Gil, and E. Sánchez. Influence of multisensory feedback on haptic accessibility tasks. Virtual Reality, 10(1):31--40, May 2006.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  7. D. E. DiFranco, G. L. Beauregard, and M. A. Srinivasan. The effect of auditory cues on the haptic perception of stiffness in virtual environments. In ASME Dynamic Systems and Control Division, volume 61, pages 17--22. ASME, 1997.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Y. Guiard. Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: The kinematic chain as a model. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19:486--517, 1987.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  9. D. Hecht and M. Reiner. Sensory dominance in combinations of audio, visual and haptic stimuli. Experimental brain research, 193:307--314, Jan 2009.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  10. D. Hecht, M. Reiner, and A. Karni. Enhancement of response times to bi- and tri-modal sensory stimuli during active movements. Experimental brain research, 185(4):655--665, Mar. 2008.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. M. A. Heller, J. A. Calcaterra, S. L. Green, and L. Brown. Intersensory conflict between vision and touch: the response modality dominates when precise, attention-riveting judgments are required. Perception & psychophysics, 61(7):1384--1398, Oct. 1999.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  12. M. Hershenson. Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation. Journal of experimental psychology, 63:289--293, Mar. 1962.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. E. Hoggan, T. Kaaresoja, P. Laitinen, and S. Brewster. Crossmodal congruence: the look, feel and sound of touchscreen widgets. Proc. IMCI '08, pages 157--164, Oct. 2008. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  14. R. Kinkead and B. Gonzalez. Human factors design recommendations for touch-operated keyboards -- final report. Technical Report 12091-FR, Honeywell, Inc., March 1969.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  15. R. Klatzky and S. J. Lederman. Touch. In I. B. Weiner, editor, Handbook of Psychology, pages 147--176. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  16. R. Klatzky and S. J. Lederman. Object Recognition by Touch. In Blindness and Brain Plasticity in Navigation and Object Perception, pages 185--207. 2008.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  17. A. Kohlrausch and S. van de Par. Auditory-visual interaction: from fundamental research in cognitive psychology to (possible) applications. In Proc. SPIE, pages 34--44, 1999.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  18. A. Lécuyer, J.-M. Burkhardt, S. Coquillart, and P. Coiffet. "boundary of illusion:" an experiment of sensory integration with a pseudo-haptic system. In Proc. VR'01, VR '01, pages 115--122, Washington, DC, USA, 2001. IEEE Computer Society. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  19. S. Lee and S. Zhai. The performance of touch screen soft buttons. In ACM CHI '09, pages 309--318, 2009. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  20. V. Levenshtein. Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals. Problems in Information Transmission, pages 8--17, 1965.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  21. J. R. Lewis, K. M. Potosnak, and R. L. Magyar. Keys and Keyboards, chapter 54, pages 1285--1315. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1997.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  22. J. Long. Effects of delayed irregular feedback on unskilled and skilled keying performance. Ergonomics, 19(2):183--202, 1976.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. T. H. Massie and J. K. Salisbury. The phantom haptic interface: A device for probing virtual objects. In ASME Winter Annual Meeting, 1994.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. H. McGurk and J. MacDonald. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588):746--748, Dec. 1976.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Microsoft. QueryPerformanceCounter. http://msdn.microsoft.com/en-us/library/windows/desktop/ms644904, March 2012.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. N. Miner, B. Gillespie, and T. Caudell. Examining the influence of audio and visual stimuli on a haptic display. In Proc. IMAGE '96, 1996.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  27. A. Nashel and S. Razzaque. Tactile virtual buttons for mobile devices. In ACM CHI '03, pages 854--855, 2003. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  28. S. Oviatt. Multimodal Interfaces, chapter 21, pages 413--432. CRC Press, 2007.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  29. D. Pollard and M. Cooper. The effect of feedback on keying performance. Applied Ergonomics, 10(4):194--200, 1979.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  30. C. J. Roe, W. H. Muto, and T. Blake. Feedback and key discrimination on membrane keypads. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 28:277--281, 1984.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  31. C. Spence, F. Pavani, and J. Driver. Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. J. of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(4):1298--1319, 2000.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. M. A. Srinivasan, G. L. Beauregard, and D. L. Brock. The impact of visual information on the haptic perception of stiffness in virtual environments. In ASME Winter Annual Meeting, volume 58, pages 555--559, 1996.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  33. H. S. Vitense, J. A. Jacko, and V. K. Emery. Multimodal feedback: an assessment of performance and mental workload. Ergonomics, 46(1--3):68--87, 2003.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  34. R. A. Wagner and M. J. Fischer. The String-to-String Correction Problem. Journal of the ACM, 21(1):168--173, 1974. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  35. P. Walker and S. Smith. Stroop interference based on the multimodal correlates of haptic size and auditory pitch. Perception, 14(6):729--736, 1985.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  36. C. D. Wickens. Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2):159--177, 2002.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref

Index Terms

  1. Effects of modality on virtual button motion and performance

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in
    • Published in

      cover image ACM Conferences
      ICMI '12: Proceedings of the 14th ACM international conference on Multimodal interaction
      October 2012
      636 pages
      ISBN:9781450314671
      DOI:10.1145/2388676

      Copyright © 2012 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 22 October 2012

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • research-article

      Acceptance Rates

      Overall Acceptance Rate453of1,080submissions,42%

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader